Social desirability, personality questionnaires, and the “better than average” effect

Abstract It is well established that respondents are much more likely to rate themselves highly on personality questionnaire items with high social desirability (SD) than on items with low SD. However, conflicting explanations have been offered for this phenomenon. In the present study, 286 participants were randomly assigned to four groups that rated 119 items from two well-known personality questionnaires. One group of participants rated themselves, a second group rated their family and friends, a third rated “people in general,” and a fourth rated the items’ SD. It was found that mean SD ratings of personality items were highly correlated with mean self-ratings and with mean ratings of family and friends for the same items (all r s > .800), but not with mean ratings of “people in general.” In other words, participants strongly tended to rate themselves, their family, and their friends as high on socially desirable qualities, but this tendency did not extend to ratings of people in general. These results support the conclusion that respondents’ personality ratings of themselves, their family and friends, but not of people in general, are influenced by the form of self-serving bias known as the “better than average effect.”

[1]  English, French, and Italian Adjective Check Lists , 1981 .

[2]  Robert Hogan,et al.  The construct validity of social desirability. , 1990 .

[3]  A. L. Edwards Relationship between probability of endorsement and social desirability scale value for a set of 2,824 personality statements. , 1966, The Journal of applied psychology.

[4]  A. L. Edwards,et al.  Social desirability and the frequency of social-reinforcement scale. , 1988 .

[5]  A. L. Edwards Construct validity and social desirability. , 1990 .

[6]  R. McGrath,et al.  Evidence for response bias as a source of error variance in applied assessment. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  C. Hanley Social desirability and responses to items from three MMPI scales: D, Sc, and K. , 1956 .

[8]  L. Clark Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality (SNAP). , 1993 .

[9]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Self-Deception and Impression Management in Test Responses , 1986 .

[10]  C. L. Cox,et al.  Positive illusion in close relationships , 1998 .

[11]  Social desirability and the description of others. , 1959, Journal of abnormal and social psychology.

[12]  Joachim I. Krueger,et al.  The Self in Social Judgment , 2013 .

[13]  Xiao-Li Meng,et al.  Comparing correlated correlation coefficients , 1992 .

[14]  Olesya Govorun,et al.  Better-than-Average Effect , 2020, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[15]  A. L. Edwards The social desirability variable in personality assessment and research , 1958 .

[16]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  Do people really believe they are above average , 2008 .

[17]  Donald H. Kluemper,et al.  Overclaiming as a measure of faking , 2011 .

[18]  J. Walsh Comment on social desirability. , 1990 .

[19]  A. L. Edwards The measurement of personality traits by scales and inventories , 1970 .

[20]  Allen L. Edwards,et al.  The Relationship Between the Judged Desirability of a Trait and the Probability That the Trait Will Be Endorsed , 1953 .

[21]  S. Iwawaki,et al.  Probability of Endorsement of Items in the Yatabe-Guilford Personality Inventory: Replication , 1966, Psychological reports.

[22]  J. Suls,et al.  Flawed Self-Assessment , 2004, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[23]  F. Fincham,et al.  Longitudinal relations between forgiveness and conflict resolution in marriage. , 2007, Journal of family psychology : JFP : journal of the Division of Family Psychology of the American Psychological Association.

[24]  K. D. Smith,et al.  Social desirability of personality items as a predictor of endorsement: a cross-cultural analysis. , 1993, The Journal of social psychology.

[25]  J. G. Holmes,et al.  The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and the construction of satisfaction in close relationships. , 1996 .

[26]  Thomas F. Pettigrew,et al.  The Ultimate Attribution Error: Extending Allport's Cognitive Analysis of Prejudice , 1979 .