Future planning: default network activity couples with frontoparietal control network and reward-processing regions during process and outcome simulations.

We spend much of our daily lives imagining how we can reach future goals and what will happen when we attain them. Despite the prevalence of such goal-directed simulations, neuroimaging studies on planning have mainly focused on executive processes in the frontal lobe. This experiment examined the neural basis of process simulations, during which participants imagined themselves going through steps toward attaining a goal, and outcome simulations, during which participants imagined events they associated with achieving a goal. In the scanner, participants engaged in these simulation tasks and an odd/even control task. We hypothesized that process simulations would recruit default and frontoparietal control network regions, and that outcome simulations, which allow us to anticipate the affective consequences of achieving goals, would recruit default and reward-processing regions. Our analysis of brain activity that covaried with process and outcome simulations confirmed these hypotheses. A functional connectivity analysis with posterior cingulate, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior inferior parietal lobule seeds showed that their activity was correlated during process simulations and associated with a distributed network of default and frontoparietal control network regions. During outcome simulations, medial prefrontal cortex and amygdala seeds covaried together and formed a functional network with default and reward-processing regions.

[1]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Doing what we imagine: Completion rates and frequency attributes of imagined future events one year after prospection , 2013, Memory.

[2]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Intrinsic Architecture Underlying the Relations among the Default, Dorsal Attention, and Frontoparietal Control Networks of the Human Brain , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain , 2012, Neuron.

[4]  D. Schacter Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. , 2012, The American psychologist.

[5]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Default network modulation and large-scale network interactivity in healthy young and old adults. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Jessica R. Andrews-Hanna,et al.  The Brain’s Default Network and Its Adaptive Role in Internal Mentation , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[7]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  The Fallacy of a “Task-Negative” Network , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[8]  Kimberly L. Ray,et al.  Meta-analytic evidence for a superordinate cognitive control network subserving diverse executive functions , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[9]  Kevin S. Brown,et al.  Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal–parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought , 2012, Brain Research.

[10]  Wei Gao,et al.  Frontal parietal control network regulates the anti‐correlated default and dorsal attention networks , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[11]  L. Cameron,et al.  Promoting physical activity with goal-oriented mental imagery: a randomized controlled trial , 2012, Journal of Behavioral Medicine.

[12]  J. Smallwood,et al.  Back to the future: Autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Influence of Aging on the Neural Correlates of Autobiographical, Episodic, and Semantic Memory Retrieval , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex subserves diverse forms of self-reflection , 2011, Social neuroscience.

[16]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review , 2011, NeuroImage.

[17]  R. Benoit,et al.  A Neural Mechanism Mediating the Impact of Episodic Prospection on Farsighted Decisions , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Arnaud D'Argembeau,et al.  Tracking the construction of episodic future thoughts. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Solving future problems: Default network and executive activity associated with goal-directed mental simulations , 2011, NeuroImage.

[20]  Jin Fan,et al.  Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  Jessica Schirmer,et al.  Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Intertemporal Choice , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  M. Linden,et al.  Frequency, characteristics and functions of future‐oriented thoughts in daily life , 2011 .

[23]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  The Prefrontal Cortex Modulates Category Selectivity in Human Extrastriate Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  C. Büchel,et al.  Neural representations of subjective reward value , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[25]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[26]  Steve Majerus,et al.  The Neural Basis of Personal Goal Processing When Envisioning Future Events , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  R. Nathan Spreng,et al.  Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Jan Peters,et al.  Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions , 2010, Neuron.

[29]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[30]  Boris Suchan,et al.  Foreseeing the future: Occurrence probability of imagined future events modulates hippocampal activation , 2009, Hippocampus.

[31]  J. Tangney,et al.  Social psychological foundations of clinical psychology , 2010 .

[32]  Gabriele Oettingen,et al.  Strategies of setting and implementing goals: Mental contrasting and implementation intentions , 2010 .

[33]  Dominic J. Packer,et al.  Neural correlates of reflection on goal states: The role of regulatory focus and temporal distance , 2009, Social neuroscience.

[34]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  H. Aarts,et al.  Planning is for doing: Implementation intentions go beyond the mere creation of goal-directed associations , 2009 .

[36]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  Cortical Hubs Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity: Mapping, Assessment of Stability, and Relation to Alzheimer's Disease , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Peter Fransson,et al.  The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network: Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis , 2008, NeuroImage.

[39]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[40]  P. Boyer Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  R. Lebow The Future of Memory , 2008 .

[42]  M. Botvinick Hierarchical models of behavior and prefrontal function , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[43]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[44]  Zhong-Lin Lu,et al.  Neural correlates of envisioning emotional events in the near and far future , 2008, NeuroImage.

[45]  E. Phelps,et al.  Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias , 2007, Nature.

[46]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Prospection: Experiencing the Future , 2007, Science.

[47]  D. Schacter,et al.  Remembering the past to imagine the future: the prospective brain , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  R. Buckner,et al.  Self-projection and the brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  David Dunning,et al.  Prediction: The inside view. , 2007 .

[51]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  The temporal distribution of past and future autobiographical events across the lifespan , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[52]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Imaging valuation models in human choice. , 2006, Annual review of neuroscience.

[53]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[54]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes , 2006 .

[55]  R. Morris,et al.  The Cognitive Psychology of Planning , 2004 .

[56]  A. R. McIntosh,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of event-related fMRI data using partial least squares , 2004, NeuroImage.

[57]  Jane F. Banfield,et al.  Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[58]  A. Damasio,et al.  Role of the Amygdala in Decision‐Making , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[59]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[60]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Imaging the mental components of a planning task , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[61]  Craig E. L. Stark,et al.  When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  Sarah E. MacPherson,et al.  Midlife aging, open-ended planning, and laboratory measures of executive function. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[63]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[65]  W. Schultz Multiple reward signals in the brain , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[66]  H. Damasio,et al.  Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[67]  T. Shallice,et al.  The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  J. MacKinnon,et al.  Bootstrap tests: how many bootstraps? , 2000 .

[69]  A. Dagher,et al.  Mapping the network for planning: a correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[70]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Mapping cognition to the brain through neural interactions. , 1999, Memory.

[71]  J. Bargh,et al.  The psychology of action : linking cognition and motivation to behavior , 1999 .

[72]  P. Gollwitzer Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .

[73]  Gregory P. Lee,et al.  Different Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex to Decision-Making , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[74]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  The Effect of Mental Simulation on Goal-Directed Performance , 1999 .

[75]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  From Thought to Action: Effects of Process-Versus Outcome-Based Mental Simulations on Performance , 1999 .

[76]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Harnessing the imagination. Mental simulation, self-regulation, and coping. , 1998, The American psychologist.

[77]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .

[78]  R. Dolan,et al.  Neural systems engaged by planning: a PET study of the Tower of London task , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[79]  J. V. Haxby,et al.  Spatial Pattern Analysis of Functional Brain Images Using Partial Least Squares , 1996, NeuroImage.

[80]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Mental simulation, motivation, and action. , 1996 .

[81]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[82]  R. Buehler,et al.  Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why people underestimate their task completion times. , 1994 .

[83]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[84]  P W Burgess,et al.  Unilateral frontal lobectomy can produce strategy application disorder. , 1993, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[85]  I. Ajzen The theory of planned behavior , 1991 .

[86]  Sherry K. Schneider,et al.  Coping and the Simulation of Events , 1989 .

[87]  W. M. Cox,et al.  Dimensions of Thought Flow in Everyday Life , 1987 .

[88]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[89]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation , 1985, Neurology.

[90]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  The Bootstrap Method for Assessing Statistical Accuracy , 1985 .

[91]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[92]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[93]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[94]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  A Cognitive Model of Planning , 1979, Cogn. Sci..

[95]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[96]  A. Battersby Plans and the Structure of Behavior , 1968 .

[97]  A. R. Lurii︠a︡ Human brain and psychological processes , 1966 .

[98]  G. Miller,et al.  Plans and the structure of behavior , 1960 .

[99]  W. Penfield,et al.  THE FRONTAL LOBE IN MAN: A CLINICAL STUDY OF MAXIMUM REMOVALS , 1935 .

[100]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.