Reactivity to novelty in cognitively-impaired and cognitively-unimpaired aged rats and young rats

Two distinct populations of aged, Long-Evans rats can be identified on the basis of performance in the Morris water maze task. Aged (24 month) unimpaired rats perform similarly to young (six month) animals. Aged, impaired rats display latencies to find the submerged platform greater than two standard deviations from the mean of the young animals. A hallmark of efficient cognitive processing is the ability to cope with environmental change. Consequently, the present studies were conducted to assess if aged, impaired animals display differential reactivity to repeated exposure to novel stimuli. Reactivity was assessed by examining the degree of (i) consumption of a novel gustatory/olfactory stimulus (sweetened milk), (ii) pain inhibition induced by exposure to a novel hot-plate (48.5 degrees C) apparatus and (iii) exploratory behaviour in an elevated plus maze and a novel open field. Aged, impaired rats exhibited lower milk consumption on day one and protracted reactivity (lower consumption over days two to eight) in comparison to aged, unimpaired and young animals. Aged, impaired rats were more reactive to novelty on the hot plate test (as indicated by longer paw lick latencies); this novelty-induced pain inhibition did not habituate in aged, impaired rats following repeated plate exposures. The degree of exploratory behaviour in both the plus maze and the open field was reduced in aged, impaired rats. This effect was not entirely a consequence of deficient affective mechanisms, as measures of anxiety (e.g., time in open arms, time in inner squares) were not different among aged impaired, aged unimpaired and young animals. These results are the first to demonstrate that behavioural deficits observed in aged, impaired animals extend beyond the impairments observed in the water maze. This behavioural profile is attributed, in part, to heightened anxiety. In addition, the impairments observed in aged, impaired animals may also reflect a reduced sensitivity to the positive incentive properties of novel stimuli.

[1]  S. File Interactions of anxiolytic and antidepressant drugs with hormones of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. , 1990, Pharmacology & therapeutics.

[2]  D. Bindra A theory of intelligent behavior , 1976 .

[3]  J. Delacour,et al.  Short-term memory, exploration and locomotor activity in aged rats , 1987, Neurobiology of Aging.

[4]  B. J. Winer Statistical Principles in Experimental Design , 1992 .

[5]  J. Aggleton,et al.  Working memory in aged rats. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[6]  M. Meaney,et al.  Ionotropic glutamate receptor subtypes in the aged memory-impaired and unimpaired Long–Evans rat , 1996, Neuroscience.

[7]  H. Hodges Maze procedures: the radial-arm and water maze compared. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  A. Parent,et al.  Facilitation of acetylcholine release and cognitive performance by an M(2)-muscarinic receptor antagonist in aged memory-impaired , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  W. Beatty,et al.  Preservation of accurate spatial memory in aged rats , 1985, Neurobiology of Aging.

[10]  J. Stewart,et al.  Activation and expression of endogenous pain control mechanisms in rats given repeated nociceptive tests under the influence of naloxone. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[11]  M. Fanselow Conditioned Fear‐Induced Opiate Analgesia: A Competing Motivational State Theory of Stress Analgesia a , 1986, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Montgomery Kc Exploratory behavior as a function of similarity of stimulus situations. , 1953 .

[13]  K. Matthews,et al.  Sucrose consumption as an hedonic measure following chronic unpredictable mild stress , 1995, Physiology & Behavior.

[14]  P. Soubrié,et al.  [Effects of antianxiety drugs on the water intake in trained and untrained rats and mice (author's transl)]. , 1975, Psychopharmacology.

[15]  C. S. Hall,et al.  Emotional behavior in the rat. I. Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality. , 1934 .

[16]  Rodgers Rj,et al.  An ethological analysis of the effects of chlordiazepoxide and bretazenil (Ro 16-6028) in the murine elevated plus-maze. , 1993 .

[17]  B. Doty Effects of handling on learning of young and aged rats. , 1968, Journal of gerontology.

[18]  A. Björklund,et al.  Regional changes in brain glucose metabolism reflect cognitive impairments in aged rats , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[19]  S. Gauthier,et al.  Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in aged, cognitively impaired and cognitively unimpaired rats , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  H F HARLOW,et al.  Mice, monkeys, men, and motives. , 1953, Psychological review.

[21]  R. Quirion,et al.  Dopamine D1, receptor ligands modulate cognitive performance and hippocampal acetylcholine release in memory-impaired aged rats , 1995, Neuroscience.

[22]  Michela Gallagher,et al.  An age-related spatial learning deficit: Choline uptake distinguishes “impaired” and “unimpaired” rats , 1988, Neurobiology of Aging.

[23]  F. Gage,et al.  Cholinergic septal grafts into the hippocampal formation improve spatial learning and memory in aged rats by an atropine-sensitive mechanism , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Gallagher,et al.  Hippocampal 3H-CPP binding and spatial learning deficits in aged rats , 1990, Psychobiology.

[25]  S. File,et al.  Validation of open : closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat , 1985, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[26]  J. Stewart,et al.  Naloxone-induced hypoalgesia: Lack of involvement of the GABA-benzodiazepine receptor complex , 1992, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.