Language comprehension is sensitive to changes in the reliability of lexical cues

Language comprehension is sensitive to changes in the reliability of lexical cues Alex B. Fine (afine@bcs.rochester.edu) Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Department of Linguistics, University of Rochester Rochester, NY 14627 USA T. Florian Jaeger (fjaeger@bcs.rochester.edu) Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Department of Computer Science, University of Rochester Rochester, NY 14627 USA Abstract This paper tests the hypothesis that language comprehen- ders update their beliefs about the statistics of their language throughout the lifespan, and that this belief update allows com- prehenders to combine probabilistic linguistic cues according to their reliability. We conduct a multi-day sentence compre- hension study in which the reliability of a probabilistic cue to syntactic structure is manipulated between subjects. We find that as the reliability of one cue to syntactic structure de- creases, comprehenders come to rely more on a second cue to syntactic structure. The results are consonant with ratio- nal models of cue integration in speech perception and in non- linguistic domains, thus suggesting a unifying computational principle governing the way humans use information across both perceptual and higher-level cognitive tasks. Keywords: psycholinguistics; adaptation; sentence process- ing; cue combination The lawyer acknowledged the judge had been unfair to the defendant. Probabilistic cues provide comprehenders with informa- tion that can guide inferences during incremental language processing, contributing to processing efficiency (see Smith & Levy, 2008 for an explicit proposal along these lines). How- ever, the cues relevant to comprehension are moving targets: probabilistic cues such as those mentioned above are context- dependent in that their validity (Bates & MacWhinney, 1987) may vary depending on speaker identity, context, and speaker dialect (see Tagliamonte, 2005 for a discussion of variability in syntax). Bates and MacWhinney (1987) define cue validity as the product of cue availability (how often a cue is present in the environment) and cue reliability (how often a cue leads to the correct inference, when present). How do comprehen- ders cope with this variability and maximize the usefulness of probabilistic cues? The current study addresses this ques- tion and tests a two-pronged hypothesis, framed in the spirit of rational analysis (Anderson, 1990): Introduction In order to process language, humans must make inferences about intended messages in the face of uncertainty arising from noisy perceptual data and ambiguity inherent in the lin- guistic signal. Research in psycholinguistics suggests that hu- mans accomplish this task partially by capitalizing on prob- abilistic cues in the linguistic as well as the non-linguistic context (Jurafsky, 1996; Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eber- hard, & Sedivy, 1995). For instance, in sentences such as (1), the judge is temporarily interpretable as both the direct object of acknowledged and the subject of an embedded sentence complement. By-word reading times at the point at which the sentence is disambiguated (had been) are correlated with the conditional probability of the structural representation as- signed to the incremental parse given a number of probabilis- tic cues. One such cue is the verb—the probability of a sen- tence complement occurring is greater given assert than ac- knowledge, based on corpus statistics and norming data (e.g., Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Kello, 1993; MacDonald, Pearlmut- ter, & Seidenberg, 1994). A second cue is the post-verbal noun phrase—if a post-verbal noun phrase is unlikely to be a direct object of the verb, it is more likely to be the subject of an embedded clause, thereby increasing the probability of a sentence complement continuation (Garnsey, Pearlmutter, Myers, & Lotocky, 1997). Finally, if present, the comple- mentizer that (e.g., The lawyer acknowledged that the judge had been . . . ) also serves as a strong cue to syntactic struc- ture. Indeed, comprehenders have been shown to rely on all of these cues during the incremental processing of sentences such as (1) (MacDonald et al., 1994). • A: Lifelong implicit learning: Throughout adulthood, hu- mans continuously update and adjust estimates of proba- bilistic cues relevant to language comprehension. We will refer to the results of this process as adaptation (cf. also Chang, Dell, & Bock, 2006 and references therein). • B: Rational linguistic adaptation: Adaptation is rational in the sense that humans update the weight they assign to a particular cue based on changes in the validity of that cue. Preliminary evidence for (A) comes from language com- prehension studies at multiple levels of representation (at the phonetic level: Clayards, Tanenhaus, Aslin, & Jacobs, 2008; Kraljic & Samuel, 2007; at the syntactic level: Fine, Qian, Jaeger, & Jacobs, 2010; Wells, Christiansen, Race, Acheson, & MacDonald, 2009). Preliminary evidence for (B) comes primarily from speech perception (Clayards et al., 2008; Kraljic, Samuel, & Brennan, 2008), though these studies are not necessarily framed in terms of the hypotheses presented above. Of particular relevance is Clayards et al. (2008), who manipulated participants’ experience with voice-onset time (VOT), a probabilistic cue to phonetic category membership. For participants in one group, the distribution over VOT val- ues that emerged over the course of the experiment had a low variance; for participants in the other group, this distribution had a high variance. The rationale of the manipulation is that

[1]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Susan M. Garnsey,et al.  The Contributions of Verb Bias and Plausibility to the Comprehension of Temporarily Ambiguous Sentences , 1997 .

[3]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Finding Structure in Time , 1990, Cogn. Sci..

[4]  Daniel Jurafsky,et al.  A Probabilistic Model of Lexical and Syntactic Access and Disambiguation , 1996, Cogn. Sci..

[5]  Nathaniel J. Smith,et al.  Optimal Processing Times in Reading: A Formal Model and Empirical Investigation , 2008 .

[6]  J. Saunders,et al.  Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant? , 2003, Vision Research.

[7]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Subject Terms: Linguistics Language Eyes & eyesight Cognition & reasoning , 1995 .

[8]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Long-term structural priming affects subsequent patterns of language production , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[9]  K. Rayner,et al.  Eye movement evidence that readers maintain and act on uncertainty about past linguistic input , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  P. Bertelson,et al.  Visual recalibration and selective adaptation in auditory–visual speech perception: Contrasting build-up courses , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  Cue Integration in Categorical Tasks: Insights from Audio-Visual Speech Perception , 2011, PloS one.

[12]  A. Samuel,et al.  Perceptual adjustments to multiple speakers , 2007 .

[13]  Sali A. Tagliamonte,et al.  No momentary fancy! The zero ‘complementizer’ in English dialects , 2005, English Language and Linguistics.

[14]  A. Samuel,et al.  First Impressions and Last Resorts , 2008, Psychological science.

[15]  R. Jacobs,et al.  Perception of speech reflects optimal use of probabilistic speech cues , 2008, Cognition.

[16]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[17]  Bob McMurray,et al.  Cue Integration With Categories: Weighting Acoustic Cues in Speech Using Unsupervised Learning and Distributional Statistics , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[18]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Character of Thought , 1990 .

[19]  Patrick Juola,et al.  A connectionist model of english past tense and plural morphology , 1999, Cogn. Sci..

[20]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  Maryellen C. MacDonald,et al.  The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution , 1994 .

[22]  Ting Qian,et al.  Syntactic Adaptation in Language Comprehension , 2010, CMCL@ACL.

[23]  Béatrice de Gelder,et al.  Visual recalibration of auditory speech versus selective speech adaptation: different build-up courses , 2004, INTERSPEECH.

[24]  G. Dell,et al.  Becoming syntactic. , 2006, Psychological review.

[25]  Patrick Juola,et al.  A connectionist model of English past tense and plural morphology , 1999 .

[26]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. , 1993 .