Affected by smells? Environmental chemical responsivity predicts odor perception.

Strong negative reactions, physical symptoms, and behavioral disruptions due to environmental odors are common in the adult population. We investigated relationships among such environmental chemosensory responsivity (CR), personality traits, affective states, and odor perception. Study 1 showed that CR and neuroticism were positively correlated in a sample of young adults (n = 101), suggesting that persons high in neuroticism respond more negatively to environmental odors. Study 2 explored the relationships among CR, noise responsivity (NR), neuroticism, and odor perception (i.e., pleasantness and intensity) in a subset of participants (n = 40). High CR was associated with high NR. Regression analyses indicated that high CR predicted higher odor intensity ratings and low olfactory threshold (high sensitivity) predicted lower pleasantness ratings. However, neuroticism was not directly associated with odor ratings or thresholds. Overall, the results suggest that CR and odor thresholds predict perceptual ratings of odors and that high CR is associated with nonchemosensory affective traits.

[1]  F. Rist,et al.  Symptom patterns, and perceptual and cognitive styles in subjects with multiple chemical sensitivity (MCS) , 2004 .

[2]  L. A. Pervin Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research , 1992 .

[3]  S. Srivastava,et al.  The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. , 1999 .

[4]  S. Nordin,et al.  Coping strategies, social support and responsibility in chemical intolerance. , 2010, Journal of clinical nursing.

[5]  G. Rubin,et al.  Multiple chemical sensitivities: A systematic review of provocation studies. , 2006, The Journal of allergy and clinical immunology.

[6]  Odor perception: multiple chemical sensitivities, chronic fatigue, and asthma. , 2000, Journal of occupational and environmental medicine.

[7]  Jonas K. Olofsson,et al.  Chemosensory perception and event-related potentials in self-reported chemical hypersensitivity. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[8]  S. Nordin,et al.  The Chemical Sensitivity Scale: Psychometric properties and comparison with the noise sensitivity scale , 2003 .

[9]  S. Mineka,et al.  Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[10]  Hans Förstl,et al.  Self-reported chemical sensitivity in Germany: a population-based survey. , 2005, International journal of hygiene and environmental health.

[11]  R. Mccaffrey,et al.  Multiple Chemical Sensitivity: A Review of the Theoretical and Research Literature , 2000, Neuropsychology Review.

[12]  R R Neutra,et al.  Prevalence of people reporting sensitivities to chemicals in a population-based survey. , 1999, American journal of epidemiology.

[13]  G. Schwartz,et al.  An olfactory-limbic model of multiple chemical sensitivity syndrome: Possible relationships to kindling and affective spectrum disorders , 1992, Biological Psychiatry.

[14]  T. Hummel,et al.  'Sniffin' sticks': olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. , 1997, Chemical senses.

[15]  S. Pouliot,et al.  A rose by any other name: would it smell as sweet? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  John B. Campbell Extraversion and noise sensitivity: a replication of Dornic and Ekehammar's study , 1992 .

[17]  A. Steinemann,et al.  Prevalence of multiple chemical sensitivities: a population-based study in the southeastern United States. , 2004, American journal of public health.

[18]  R. Larsen,et al.  Personality and susceptibility to positive and negative emotional states. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  Philip J. Corr,et al.  The role of neuroticism in startle reactions to fearful and disgusting stimuli. , 2000 .

[20]  R. Larsen,et al.  Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative affect: A test of two theoretical models , 1997 .

[21]  S. Nordin,et al.  Prevalence and risk factors for chemical sensitivity and sensory hyperreactivity in teenagers. , 2008, International journal of hygiene and environmental health.

[22]  J. R. Thomas,et al.  Individual differences in noise annoyance and the uncomfortable loudness level , 1982 .

[23]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[24]  O. John,et al.  Big Five Inventory , 2012, Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences.

[25]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed. , 2007 .

[26]  L. Hillert,et al.  Odor processing in multiple chemical sensitivity , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[27]  N. Weinstein Individual differences in reactions to noise: a longitudinal study in a college dormitory. , 1978, The Journal of applied psychology.

[28]  尾島 正幸 Odor perception in patients with multiple chemical sensitivity , 2003 .

[29]  P. Eelen,et al.  Acquiring Symptoms in Response to Odors: A Learning Perspective on Multiple Chemical Sensitivity , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[30]  D. Papo,et al.  Chemosensory function and psychological profile in patients with multiple chemical sensitivity: comparison with odor-sensitive and asymptomatic controls. , 2006, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[31]  H. S. Koelega Extraversion, sex, arousal and olfactory sensitivity. , 1970, Acta psychologica.

[32]  R. Doty,et al.  Olfactory sensitivity, nasal resistance, and autonomic function in patients with multiple chemical sensitivities. , 1988, Archives of otolaryngology--head & neck surgery.

[33]  B. Pause,et al.  Context effects on odor processing: An event-related potential study , 2008, NeuroImage.

[34]  S. Dornič,et al.  Extraversion, neuroticism, and noise sensitivity , 1990 .

[35]  J. M. Peterson,et al.  Self-reported illness from chemical odors in young adults without clinical syndromes or occupational exposures. , 1993, Archives of environmental health.

[36]  Paul Kline,et al.  The Big Three or the Big Five—the EPQ-R vs the NEO-PI: a research note, replication and elaboration , 1995 .

[37]  S. Issanchou,et al.  Selection of odorants for memory tests on the basis of familiarity, perceived complexity, pleasantness, similarity and identification. , 2002, Chemical senses.

[38]  Gregory J. Meyer,et al.  Structural convergence of mood and personality: Evidence for old and new directions. , 1989 .

[39]  S. Harkins,et al.  Effects of extraversion and neuroticism on experimental pain, clinical pain, and illness behavior , 1989, Pain.

[40]  B. Pause,et al.  PERSONALITY AND OLFACTORY SENSITIVITY , 1998 .

[41]  D. Watson,et al.  Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  S. Nordin,et al.  Prevalence and risk factors for self-reported odour intolerance: the Skövde population-based study , 2005, International archives of occupational and environmental health.