Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review)
暂无分享,去创建一个
M. Chetouani | D. Cohen | C. Saint-Georges | R. Cassel | F. Apicella | A. Mahdhaoui | F. Muratori | M. Laznik
[1] C. A. Ferguson. Baby Talk in Six Languages , 1964 .
[2] R. McKevitt. Talking with children. , 1970, Journal of the New York State School Nurse-Teachers Association.
[3] C. Snow. Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .
[4] J. Bohannon,et al. Children's control of adult speech. , 1977 .
[5] H. Gleitman,et al. Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .
[6] K. Nelson,et al. Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships , 1979, Journal of Child Language.
[7] C. A. Ferguson,et al. Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .
[8] Daniel N. Stern,et al. Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. , 1982 .
[9] R Treiman,et al. Doggerel: motherese in a new context , 1982, Journal of Child Language.
[10] R. D. Kavanaugh,et al. Parental Speech to Young Children: A Longitudinal Analysis. , 1982 .
[11] D. Rutter,et al. Social interaction and language acquisition: Motherese help you , 1982 .
[12] M. Papoušek,et al. Biological basis of social interactions: implications of research for an understanding of behavioural deviance. , 1983, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[13] E. Bard,et al. The unintelligibility of speech to children , 1983, Journal of Child Language.
[14] D. Stern,et al. The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.
[15] A. Fernald,et al. Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .
[16] A. Lederberg. Interaction between deaf preschoolers and unfamiliar hearing adults. , 1984, Child development.
[17] H. Gleitman,et al. The current status of the motherese hypothesis , 1984, Journal of Child Language.
[18] R G Karzon,et al. Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one- to four-month-old infants. , 1985, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.
[19] "Motherese" of Mr. Rogers: a description of the dialogue of educational television programs. , 1986, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.
[20] H. Scarborough,et al. Mother, I'd still rather do it myself: some further non-effects of ‘motherese’ , 1986, Journal of Child Language.
[21] D. Furrow,et al. A further look at the motherese hypothesis: a reply to Gleitman, Newport & Gleitman , 1986, Journal of Child Language.
[22] E. Nwokah. Maidese Versus Motherese — Is The Language Input of Child and Adult Caregivers Similar? , 1987, Language and speech.
[23] P. Kuhl,et al. Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .
[24] P. Jusczyk,et al. Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.
[25] P. Kuhl,et al. Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .
[26] D. P. Hayes,et al. Vocabulary simplification for children: a special case of ‘motherese’? , 1988, Journal of Child Language.
[27] B. Bettes,et al. Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. , 1988, Child development.
[28] K. Dyer,et al. Prosodic aspects of motherese: effects on gaze and responsiveness in developmentally disabled children. , 1988, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[29] K. Wheldall,et al. Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data , 1989, Journal of Child Language.
[30] R G Karzon,et al. Syllabic pitch perception in 2- to 3-month-old infants , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] A. Fernald,et al. A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.
[32] D. Symmes,et al. Contour variables in vocal communication between squirrel monkey mothers and infants. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.
[33] J. Werker,et al. Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: a developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.
[34] P. Jusczyk,et al. A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .
[35] A. Fernald,et al. Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.
[36] Marc H. Bornstein,et al. Infant responses to prototypical melodic contours in parental speech , 1990 .
[37] R. Aslin,et al. Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.
[38] Child- or Adult-Directed Speech and Esteem: Effects on Performance and Arousal in Elderly Adults , 1991, International journal of aging & human development.
[39] A. Fernald,et al. Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .
[40] E. Hoff-Ginsberg,et al. Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. , 1991, Child development.
[41] H. Papoušek,et al. The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages , 1991 .
[42] Yoko Shimura,et al. Sound Spectrographic Studies on the Relation Between Motherese and Pleasure Vocalization in Early Infancy , 1992, Acta paediatrica Japonica : Overseas edition.
[43] L B Leonard,et al. Vowel duration in mothers' speech to young children. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[44] Nobuo Masataka,et al. Motherese in a signed language , 1992 .
[45] J. Werker,et al. Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants , 1992 .
[46] A. Fernald. Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. , 1992 .
[47] P. McLeod,et al. What Studies of Communication with Infants Ask Us about Psychology: Baby-Talk and Other Speech Registers , 1993 .
[48] K. Nelson,et al. The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition , 1993, Journal of Child Language.
[49] The effect of prosody on young infants' speech perception , 1993 .
[50] Laryngographic analysis of child-directed speech. , 1993, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.
[51] P. Hepper,et al. Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice , 1993 .
[52] A. Fernald. Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. , 1993, Child development.
[53] R. Aslin,et al. Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.
[54] L. Leonard,et al. Duration of function-word vowels in mothers' speech to young children. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.
[55] M. Owren,et al. Dishabituation of visual attention in 4-month-olds by infant-directed frequency sweeps , 1994 .
[56] C. Fisher,et al. The Given-New Contract in Speech to Infants , 1995 .
[57] John Colombo,et al. Infants' detection of analogs of 'motherese' in noise , 1995 .
[58] M. Goldstein,et al. Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. , 1995, Developmental psychobiology.
[59] R. Golinkoff,et al. Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition , 1995, Journal of Child Language.
[60] James L. Morgan,et al. Prosody and the Roots of Parsing , 1996 .
[61] J. Cohn,et al. A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. , 1996, Child development.
[62] U Bellugi,et al. Competition on the face: affect and language in ASL motherese , 1996, Journal of Child Language.
[63] Catharine H. Echols,et al. Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech , 1996 .
[64] P. Kaplan,et al. Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for faces , 1996 .
[65] P. Kaplan,et al. Infant-Directed versus Adult-Directed Speech as Signals for Faces. , 1996 .
[66] Jane L. Abraham,et al. The development of infants' preference for motherese , 1997 .
[67] C. Best,et al. Accommodation in mean f0 during mother–infant and father–infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study , 1997, Journal of Child Language.
[68] A. Walker-Andrews. Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.
[69] L. Trainor,et al. Infants prefer higher-pitched singing , 1998 .
[70] N. Masataka,et al. Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.
[71] M. Monnot,et al. Function of infant-directed speech , 1999, Human nature.
[72] M. C. Caselli,et al. Gesturing in mother-child interactions * , 1999 .
[73] J. Bachorowski,et al. Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. , 1999, Child development.
[74] Nobuo Masataka,et al. A variable that may affect individual differences in the child‐directed speech of Japanese women , 1999 .
[75] A. Fogel,et al. The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communication: a dynamic systems perspective. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[76] K. Wheldall,et al. Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British fathers to their children , 1999 .
[77] N. Reissland,et al. Turn-taking in early vocal interaction: a comparison of premature and term infants' vocal interaction with their mothers. , 1999, Child: care, health and development.
[78] L. Trainor,et al. Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.
[79] L. Gogate,et al. A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.
[80] Anne Fernald,et al. Speech to Infants as Hyperspeech: Knowledge-Driven Processes in Early Word Recognition , 2000, Phonetica.
[81] S. Trehub,et al. Emotion and music in infancy , 2001 .
[82] D. Burnham,et al. Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .
[83] J. Bachorowski,et al. Role of Clinical Diagnosis and Medication Use in Effects of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[84] K. Wheldall,et al. How Do Grandmothers Speak to Their Grandchildren? Fundamental Frequency and Temporal Modifications in the Speech of British Grandmothers to Their Grandchildren , 2001 .
[85] S Kiritani,et al. Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[86] Katsuko Niwano,et al. Intonation Contour of Japanese Maternal Infant-Directed Speech and Infant Vocal Response , 2002 .
[87] J. Bachorowski,et al. Infants of Depressed Mothers, Although Competent Learners, Fail to Learn in Response to Their Own Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech , 2002, Psychological science.
[88] Leher Singh,et al. Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? , 2002, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[89] L. Trainor,et al. Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[90] D. Burnham,et al. What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals , 2002, Science.
[91] Dare A. Baldwin,et al. Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action , 2002 .
[92] Watch What You Say and How You Say It: Differential Response to Speech by Participants With and Without Head Injuries , 2002, Applied neuropsychology.
[93] J. Shepherd,et al. The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant , 2002 .
[94] Katsuko Niwano,et al. Acoustic Determinants Eliciting Japanese Infants' Vocal Response to Maternal Speech , 2002, Psychological reports.
[95] D. Moore,et al. Categorization of infant-directed speech: development from 4 to 6 months. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.
[96] P. Kuhl,et al. Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[97] Malcolm Slaney,et al. BabyEars: A recognition system for affective vocalizations , 2003, Speech Commun..
[98] Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions , 2003 .
[99] Rochelle S. Newman,et al. Children's use of the prosodic characteristics of infant-directed speech , 2003 .
[100] D. Fidler. Parental vocalizations and perceived immaturity in down syndrome. , 2003, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.
[101] D. Orbelo,et al. Acoustic Analyses Support Subjective Judgments of Vocal Emotion , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[102] D. Burnham,et al. Pitch and Communicative Intent in Mother's Speech: Adjustments for Age and Sex in the First Year , 2003 .
[103] P. J. Brooks,et al. Diminutivization supports gender acquisition in Russian children. , 2003, Journal of child language.
[104] Kuniaki Sugai,et al. Maternal Accommodation in Infant-Directed Speech during Mother's and Twin-Infants' Vocal Interactions , 2003, Psychological reports.
[105] J. Shepherd,et al. Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. , 2004, Journal of affective disorders.
[106] D. Falk. Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese? , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[107] P. Kaplan,et al. Infants of chronically depressed mothers learn in response to male, but not female, infant-directed speech. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[108] Shigeaki Amano,et al. Speech overlap in Japanese mother-child conversations. , 2004, Journal of child language.
[109] Sandra E. Trehub,et al. Infants' responsiveness to maternal speech and singing , 2004 .
[110] Kjellrun T. Englund. Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months , 2005 .
[111] Erika Hoff,et al. Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. , 2005, Journal of communication disorders.
[112] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation , 2005, Cognition.
[113] Paul Matychuk. The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: a case study , 2005 .
[114] P. J. Brooks,et al. Diminutives in child-directed speech supplement metric with distributional word segmentation cues , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[115] K. Bard,et al. Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. , 2005, Developmental science.
[116] Erik D. Thiessen,et al. Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[117] Dawn M Behne,et al. Infant Directed Speech in Natural Interaction—Norwegian Vowel Quantity and Quality , 2005, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[118] Steven M. Schimmel,et al. Statistical properties of infant-directed versus adult-directed speech: insights from speech recognition. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[119] T. Kondo,et al. Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech , 2006, Archives of Disease in Childhood - Fetal and Neonatal Edition.
[120] Virginia Slaughter,et al. Toddler subtraction with large sets: further evidence for an analog-magnitude representation of number. , 2006, Developmental science.
[121] Dawn M. Behne,et al. Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months , 2006 .
[122] L. D’Odorico,et al. Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.
[123] Anne Fernald,et al. Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. , 2006, Developmental science.
[124] Tomohiro Nakatani,et al. Fundamental frequency of infants' and parents' utterances in longitudinal recordings. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[125] Rachel M. Miller,et al. Mothers' Speech to Hearing‐Impaired Infants and Children With Cochlear Implants , 2006 .
[126] Rochelle S. Newman,et al. Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[127] D. Burnham,et al. Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion , 2006 .
[128] R. Feldman. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[129] P. J. Brooks,et al. Crosslinguistic evidence for the diminutive advantage: gender agreement in Russian and Serbian children. , 2007, Journal of child language.
[130] Renate Zangl,et al. Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. , 2007 .
[131] P. Kuhl,et al. Acoustic analysis of lexical tone in Mandarin infant-directed speech. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[132] J. Morgan,et al. Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. , 2007, Developmental science.
[133] Laurel Fais,et al. Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese , 2007, Cognition.
[134] L. Trainor,et al. Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[135] P. J. Brooks,et al. Diminutives facilitate word segmentation in natural speech: Cross-linguistic evidence , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[136] P. Kaplan,et al. Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.
[137] G. Gergely,et al. On pedagogy. , 2007, Developmental science.
[138] Lori J. Leibold,et al. Infant Auditory Sensitivity to Pure Tones and Frequency-Modulated Tones. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[139] P. Kuhl. Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.
[140] H Clark Barrett,et al. Recognizing Intentions in Infant-Directed Speech , 2007, Psychological science.
[141] James L. McClelland,et al. Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-directed speech , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[142] D. Ravid,et al. Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child-directed speech* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[143] Laurel J. Trainor,et al. Infant-Directed Speech Is Modulated by Infant Feedback , 2008 .
[144] M. Rowe,et al. Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[145] S. Edelman,et al. Learn locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues , 2008, Cognition.
[146] K. Stormark,et al. Prosodic Modification and Vocal Adjustments in Mothers' Speech during Face-to-Face Interaction with Their Two- to Four-Month-Old Infants: A Double Video Study. , 2008 .
[147] K. Abel,et al. Content and style of speech from mothers with schizophrenia towards their infants , 2008, Psychiatry Research.
[148] P. MacNeilage,et al. Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[149] G. Csibra,et al. Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.
[150] M. Saxton. What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[151] J. Morgan,et al. Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.
[152] L. Singh,et al. Influences of high and low variability on infant word recognition , 2008, Cognition.
[153] R. Parncutt. Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion , 2009 .
[154] C. Snowdon,et al. Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music , 2010, Biology Letters.
[155] Patricia K Kuhl,et al. Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.
[156] E. Longhi. `Songese': maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants , 2009 .
[157] P. Kaplan,et al. Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[158] Christine Kitamura,et al. Age-Specific Preferences for Infant-Directed Affective Intent. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[159] Laura Lakusta,et al. The Role of Verbal Repetition in the Development of Infant Speech Preferences From 4 to 14 Months of Age. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[160] L. Singh,et al. Influences of Infant-Directed Speech on Early Word Recognition. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[161] Katherine Demuth,et al. Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[162] Ilanit Gordon,et al. Oxytocin and the Development of Parenting in Humans , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.
[163] Alejandrina Cristià,et al. Phonetic enhancement of sibilants in infant-directed speech. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[164] W. Verhelst,et al. Tonal synchrony in mother-infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.
[165] P. Kaplan,et al. A Privileged Status for Male Infant-Directed Speech in Infants of Depressed Mothers? Role of Father Involvement. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.
[166] Shimon Edelman,et al. An empirical generative framework for computational modeling of language acquisition. , 2010, Journal of child language.
[167] Laurel Fais,et al. Now you hear it, now you don't: vowel devoicing in Japanese infant-directed speech. , 2010, Journal of child language.
[168] P. MacNeilage,et al. Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: a cross-linguistic study of Korean- and English-learning infants. , 2010, Journal of child language.
[169] James L. Morgan,et al. Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.
[170] Yana Yunusova,et al. Lip movement exaggerations during infant-directed speech. , 2010, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[171] T. Bergeson,et al. The effects of age and infant hearing status on maternal use of prosodic cues for clause boundaries in speech. , 2011, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[172] Kazuyuki Shinohara,et al. Discrimination between mothers’ infant- and adult-directed speech using hidden Markov models , 2011, Neuroscience Research.
[173] Donna Erickson,et al. Processing of infant-directed speech by adults , 2011, NeuroImage.
[174] Mohamed Chetouani,et al. Computerized home video detection for motherese may help to study impaired interaction between infants who become autistic and their parents , 2011, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.
[175] P. Kaplan,et al. An associative learning deficit in 1-year-old infants of depressed mothers: role of depression duration. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.
[176] Adena Schachner,et al. Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. , 2011, Developmental psychology.
[177] M. Gattis,et al. Infants infer intentions from prosody , 2012 .
[178] Mohamed Chetouani,et al. Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? , 2013, PloS one.