Motherese in Interaction: At the Cross-Road of Emotion and Cognition? (A Systematic Review)

Various aspects of motherese also known as infant-directed speech (IDS) have been studied for many years. As it is a widespread phenomenon, it is suspected to play some important roles in infant development. Therefore, our purpose was to provide an update of the evidence accumulated by reviewing all of the empirical or experimental studies that have been published since 1966 on IDS driving factors and impacts. Two databases were screened and 144 relevant studies were retained. General linguistic and prosodic characteristics of IDS were found in a variety of languages, and IDS was not restricted to mothers. IDS varied with factors associated with the caregiver (e.g., cultural, psychological and physiological) and the infant (e.g., reactivity and interactive feedback). IDS promoted infants’ affect, attention and language learning. Cognitive aspects of IDS have been widely studied whereas affective ones still need to be developed. However, during interactions, the following two observations were notable: (1) IDS prosody reflects emotional charges and meets infants’ preferences, and (2) mother-infant contingency and synchrony are crucial for IDS production and prolongation. Thus, IDS is part of an interactive loop that may play an important role in infants’ cognitive and social development.

[1]  C. A. Ferguson Baby Talk in Six Languages , 1964 .

[2]  R. McKevitt Talking with children. , 1970, Journal of the New York State School Nurse-Teachers Association.

[3]  C. Snow Mothers' Speech to Children Learning Language. , 1972 .

[4]  J. Bohannon,et al.  Children's control of adult speech. , 1977 .

[5]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style , 1977 .

[6]  K. Nelson,et al.  Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships , 1979, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  C. A. Ferguson,et al.  Talking to Children: Language Input and Acquisition , 1979 .

[8]  Daniel N. Stern,et al.  Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. , 1982 .

[9]  R Treiman,et al.  Doggerel: motherese in a new context , 1982, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  R. D. Kavanaugh,et al.  Parental Speech to Young Children: A Longitudinal Analysis. , 1982 .

[11]  D. Rutter,et al.  Social interaction and language acquisition: Motherese help you , 1982 .

[12]  M. Papoušek,et al.  Biological basis of social interactions: implications of research for an understanding of behavioural deviance. , 1983, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[13]  E. Bard,et al.  The unintelligibility of speech to children , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  D. Stern,et al.  The prosody of maternal speech: infant age and context related changes , 1983, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  A. Fernald,et al.  Expanded Intonation Contours in Mothers' Speech to Newborns. , 1984 .

[16]  A. Lederberg Interaction between deaf preschoolers and unfamiliar hearing adults. , 1984, Child development.

[17]  H. Gleitman,et al.  The current status of the motherese hypothesis , 1984, Journal of Child Language.

[18]  R G Karzon,et al.  Discrimination of polysyllabic sequences by one- to four-month-old infants. , 1985, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

[19]  "Motherese" of Mr. Rogers: a description of the dialogue of educational television programs. , 1986, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[20]  H. Scarborough,et al.  Mother, I'd still rather do it myself: some further non-effects of ‘motherese’ , 1986, Journal of Child Language.

[21]  D. Furrow,et al.  A further look at the motherese hypothesis: a reply to Gleitman, Newport & Gleitman , 1986, Journal of Child Language.

[22]  E. Nwokah Maidese Versus Motherese — Is The Language Input of Child and Adult Caregivers Similar? , 1987, Language and speech.

[23]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech , 1987 .

[24]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Clauses are perceptual units for young infants , 1987, Cognition.

[25]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. , 1988 .

[26]  D. P. Hayes,et al.  Vocabulary simplification for children: a special case of ‘motherese’? , 1988, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  B. Bettes,et al.  Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. , 1988, Child development.

[28]  K. Dyer,et al.  Prosodic aspects of motherese: effects on gaze and responsiveness in developmentally disabled children. , 1988, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[29]  K. Wheldall,et al.  Pitch alterations in British motherese: some preliminary acoustic data , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[30]  R G Karzon,et al.  Syllabic pitch perception in 2- to 3-month-old infants , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  A. Fernald,et al.  A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers' and fathers' speech to preverbal infants , 1989, Journal of Child Language.

[32]  D. Symmes,et al.  Contour variables in vocal communication between squirrel monkey mothers and infants. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.

[33]  J. Werker,et al.  Infant preference for both male and female infant-directed talk: a developmental study of attentional and affective responsiveness. , 1989, Canadian journal of psychology.

[34]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[35]  A. Fernald,et al.  Intonation and communicative intent in mothers' speech to infants: is the melody the message? , 1989, Child development.

[36]  Marc H. Bornstein,et al.  Infant responses to prototypical melodic contours in parental speech , 1990 .

[37]  R. Aslin,et al.  Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. , 1990, Child development.

[38]  Child- or Adult-Directed Speech and Esteem: Effects on Performance and Arousal in Elderly Adults , 1991, International journal of aging & human development.

[39]  A. Fernald,et al.  Prosody and focus in speech to infants and adults , 1991 .

[40]  E. Hoff-Ginsberg,et al.  Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. , 1991, Child development.

[41]  H. Papoušek,et al.  The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages , 1991 .

[42]  Yoko Shimura,et al.  Sound Spectrographic Studies on the Relation Between Motherese and Pleasure Vocalization in Early Infancy , 1992, Acta paediatrica Japonica : Overseas edition.

[43]  L B Leonard,et al.  Vowel duration in mothers' speech to young children. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[44]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  Motherese in a signed language , 1992 .

[45]  J. Werker,et al.  Preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech: Evidence from 7-week-old infants , 1992 .

[46]  A. Fernald Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective. , 1992 .

[47]  P. McLeod,et al.  What Studies of Communication with Infants Ask Us about Psychology: Baby-Talk and Other Speech Registers , 1993 .

[48]  K. Nelson,et al.  The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[49]  The effect of prosody on young infants' speech perception , 1993 .

[50]  Laryngographic analysis of child-directed speech. , 1993, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[51]  P. Hepper,et al.  Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice , 1993 .

[52]  A. Fernald Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. , 1993, Child development.

[53]  R. Aslin,et al.  Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. , 1994, Child development.

[54]  L. Leonard,et al.  Duration of function-word vowels in mothers' speech to young children. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[55]  M. Owren,et al.  Dishabituation of visual attention in 4-month-olds by infant-directed frequency sweeps , 1994 .

[56]  C. Fisher,et al.  The Given-New Contract in Speech to Infants , 1995 .

[57]  John Colombo,et al.  Infants' detection of analogs of 'motherese' in noise , 1995 .

[58]  M. Goldstein,et al.  Habituation, sensitization, and infants' responses to motherese speech. , 1995, Developmental psychobiology.

[59]  R. Golinkoff,et al.  Infant-directed speech facilitates lexical learning in adults hearing Chinese: implications for language acquisition , 1995, Journal of Child Language.

[60]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Prosody and the Roots of Parsing , 1996 .

[61]  J. Cohn,et al.  A combination of vocal fo dynamic and summary features discriminates between three pragmatic categories of infant-directed speech. , 1996, Child development.

[62]  U Bellugi,et al.  Competition on the face: affect and language in ASL motherese , 1996, Journal of Child Language.

[63]  Catharine H. Echols,et al.  Stressed and word-final syllables in infant-directed speech , 1996 .

[64]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infant-directed versus adult-directed speech as signals for faces , 1996 .

[65]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infant-Directed versus Adult-Directed Speech as Signals for Faces. , 1996 .

[66]  Jane L. Abraham,et al.  The development of infants' preference for motherese , 1997 .

[67]  C. Best,et al.  Accommodation in mean f0 during mother–infant and father–infant vocal interactions: a longitudinal case study , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[68]  A. Walker-Andrews Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: differentiation of multimodal information. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[69]  L. Trainor,et al.  Infants prefer higher-pitched singing , 1998 .

[70]  N. Masataka,et al.  Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-month-old hearing infants. , 1998, Developmental psychology.

[71]  M. Monnot,et al.  Function of infant-directed speech , 1999, Human nature.

[72]  M. C. Caselli,et al.  Gesturing in mother-child interactions * , 1999 .

[73]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Child-directed speech produced by mothers with symptoms of depression fails to promote associative learning in 4-month-old infants. , 1999, Child development.

[74]  Nobuo Masataka,et al.  A variable that may affect individual differences in the child‐directed speech of Japanese women , 1999 .

[75]  A. Fogel,et al.  The integration of laughter and speech in vocal communication: a dynamic systems perspective. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[76]  K. Wheldall,et al.  Fundamental frequency and temporal modifications in the speech of British fathers to their children , 1999 .

[77]  N. Reissland,et al.  Turn-taking in early vocal interaction: a comparison of premature and term infants' vocal interaction with their mothers. , 1999, Child: care, health and development.

[78]  L. Trainor,et al.  Is Infant-Directed Speech Prosody a Result of the Vocal Expression of Emotion? , 2000, Psychological science.

[79]  L. Gogate,et al.  A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.

[80]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Speech to Infants as Hyperspeech: Knowledge-Driven Processes in Early Word Recognition , 2000, Phonetica.

[81]  S. Trehub,et al.  Emotion and music in infancy , 2001 .

[82]  D. Burnham,et al.  Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and non-tonal language , 2001 .

[83]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Role of Clinical Diagnosis and Medication Use in Effects of Maternal Depression on Infant-Directed Speech. , 2001, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[84]  K. Wheldall,et al.  How Do Grandmothers Speak to Their Grandchildren? Fundamental Frequency and Temporal Modifications in the Speech of British Grandmothers to Their Grandchildren , 2001 .

[85]  S Kiritani,et al.  Developmental change in auditory preferences for speech stimuli in Japanese infants. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[86]  Katsuko Niwano,et al.  Intonation Contour of Japanese Maternal Infant-Directed Speech and Infant Vocal Response , 2002 .

[87]  J. Bachorowski,et al.  Infants of Depressed Mothers, Although Competent Learners, Fail to Learn in Response to Their Own Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech , 2002, Psychological science.

[88]  Leher Singh,et al.  Infants' Listening Preferences: Baby Talk or Happy Talk? , 2002, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[89]  L. Trainor,et al.  Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[90]  D. Burnham,et al.  What's New, Pussycat? On Talking to Babies and Animals , 2002, Science.

[91]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Evidence for ‘motionese’: modifications in mothers’ infant-directed action , 2002 .

[92]  Watch What You Say and How You Say It: Differential Response to Speech by Participants With and Without Head Injuries , 2002, Applied neuropsychology.

[93]  J. Shepherd,et al.  The melody of surprise: maternal surprise vocalizations during play with her infant , 2002 .

[94]  Katsuko Niwano,et al.  Acoustic Determinants Eliciting Japanese Infants' Vocal Response to Maternal Speech , 2002, Psychological reports.

[95]  D. Moore,et al.  Categorization of infant-directed speech: development from 4 to 6 months. , 2003, Developmental psychobiology.

[96]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[97]  Malcolm Slaney,et al.  BabyEars: A recognition system for affective vocalizations , 2003, Speech Commun..

[98]  Pitch Characteristics of Speech During Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Vocal Interactions , 2003 .

[99]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Children's use of the prosodic characteristics of infant-directed speech , 2003 .

[100]  D. Fidler Parental vocalizations and perceived immaturity in down syndrome. , 2003, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[101]  D. Orbelo,et al.  Acoustic Analyses Support Subjective Judgments of Vocal Emotion , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[102]  D. Burnham,et al.  Pitch and Communicative Intent in Mother's Speech: Adjustments for Age and Sex in the First Year , 2003 .

[103]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Diminutivization supports gender acquisition in Russian children. , 2003, Journal of child language.

[104]  Kuniaki Sugai,et al.  Maternal Accommodation in Infant-Directed Speech during Mother's and Twin-Infants' Vocal Interactions , 2003, Psychological reports.

[105]  J. Shepherd,et al.  Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. , 2004, Journal of affective disorders.

[106]  D. Falk Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese? , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[107]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infants of chronically depressed mothers learn in response to male, but not female, infant-directed speech. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[108]  Shigeaki Amano,et al.  Speech overlap in Japanese mother-child conversations. , 2004, Journal of child language.

[109]  Sandra E. Trehub,et al.  Infants' responsiveness to maternal speech and singing , 2004 .

[110]  Kjellrun T. Englund Voice onset time in infant directed speech over the first six months , 2005 .

[111]  Erika Hoff,et al.  Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. , 2005, Journal of communication disorders.

[112]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Stress changes the representational landscape: evidence from word segmentation , 2005, Cognition.

[113]  Paul Matychuk The role of child-directed speech in language acquisition: a case study , 2005 .

[114]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Diminutives in child-directed speech supplement metric with distributional word segmentation cues , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[115]  K. Bard,et al.  Maternal gestures with 20-month-old infants in two contexts. , 2005, Developmental science.

[116]  Erik D. Thiessen,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Facilitates Word Segmentation. , 2005, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[117]  Dawn M Behne,et al.  Infant Directed Speech in Natural Interaction—Norwegian Vowel Quantity and Quality , 2005, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[118]  Steven M. Schimmel,et al.  Statistical properties of infant-directed versus adult-directed speech: insights from speech recognition. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[119]  T. Kondo,et al.  Frontal cerebral blood flow change associated with infant-directed speech , 2006, Archives of Disease in Childhood - Fetal and Neonatal Edition.

[120]  Virginia Slaughter,et al.  Toddler subtraction with large sets: further evidence for an analog-magnitude representation of number. , 2006, Developmental science.

[121]  Dawn M. Behne,et al.  Changes in infant directed speech in the first six months , 2006 .

[122]  L. D’Odorico,et al.  Prosodic and lexical aspects of maternal linguistic input to late-talking toddlers. , 2006, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[123]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. , 2006, Developmental science.

[124]  Tomohiro Nakatani,et al.  Fundamental frequency of infants' and parents' utterances in longitudinal recordings. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[125]  Rachel M. Miller,et al.  Mothers' Speech to Hearing‐Impaired Infants and Children With Cochlear Implants , 2006 .

[126]  Rochelle S. Newman,et al.  Changes in Preference for Infant-Directed Speech in Low and Moderate Noise by 4.5- to 13-Month-Olds. , 2006, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[127]  D. Burnham,et al.  Slow Speech Enhances Younger But Not Older Infants' Perception of Vocal Emotion , 2006 .

[128]  R. Feldman Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. , 2007, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[129]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Crosslinguistic evidence for the diminutive advantage: gender agreement in Russian and Serbian children. , 2007, Journal of child language.

[130]  Renate Zangl,et al.  Increased brain activity to infant-directed speech in 6-and 13-month-old infants. , 2007 .

[131]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Acoustic analysis of lexical tone in Mandarin infant-directed speech. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[132]  J. Morgan,et al.  Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. , 2007, Developmental science.

[133]  Laurel Fais,et al.  Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese , 2007, Cognition.

[134]  L. Trainor,et al.  Frontal brain electrical activity (EEG) and heart rate in response to affective infant-directed (ID) speech in 9-month-old infants , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[135]  P. J. Brooks,et al.  Diminutives facilitate word segmentation in natural speech: Cross-linguistic evidence , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[136]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Infant-directed speech produced by fathers with symptoms of depression: effects on infant associative learning in a conditioned-attention paradigm. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.

[137]  G. Gergely,et al.  On pedagogy. , 2007, Developmental science.

[138]  Lori J. Leibold,et al.  Infant Auditory Sensitivity to Pure Tones and Frequency-Modulated Tones. , 2007, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[139]  P. Kuhl Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.

[140]  H Clark Barrett,et al.  Recognizing Intentions in Infant-Directed Speech , 2007, Psychological science.

[141]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Unsupervised learning of vowel categories from infant-directed speech , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[142]  D. Ravid,et al.  Prosodic patterns in Hebrew child-directed speech* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[143]  Laurel J. Trainor,et al.  Infant-Directed Speech Is Modulated by Infant Feedback , 2008 .

[144]  M. Rowe,et al.  Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[145]  S. Edelman,et al.  Learn locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues , 2008, Cognition.

[146]  K. Stormark,et al.  Prosodic Modification and Vocal Adjustments in Mothers' Speech during Face-to-Face Interaction with Their Two- to Four-Month-Old Infants: A Double Video Study. , 2008 .

[147]  K. Abel,et al.  Content and style of speech from mothers with schizophrenia towards their infants , 2008, Psychiatry Research.

[148]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Segmental properties of input to infants: a study of Korean , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[149]  G. Csibra,et al.  Gaze Following in Human Infants Depends on Communicative Signals , 2008, Current Biology.

[150]  M. Saxton What's in a name? Coming to terms with the child's linguistic environment , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[151]  J. Morgan,et al.  Acoustical cues and grammatical units in speech to two preverbal infants* , 2008, Journal of Child Language.

[152]  L. Singh,et al.  Influences of high and low variability on infant word recognition , 2008, Cognition.

[153]  R. Parncutt Prenatal and infant conditioning, the mother schema, and the origins of music and religion , 2009 .

[154]  C. Snowdon,et al.  Affective responses in tamarins elicited by species-specific music , 2010, Biology Letters.

[155]  Patricia K Kuhl,et al.  Age-related changes in acoustic modifications of Mandarin maternal speech to preverbal infants and five-year-old children: a longitudinal study* , 2009, Journal of Child Language.

[156]  E. Longhi `Songese': maternal structuring of musical interaction with infants , 2009 .

[157]  P. Kaplan,et al.  Maternal Sensitivity and the Learning-Promoting Effects of Depressed and Non-Depressed Mothers' Infant-Directed Speech. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[158]  Christine Kitamura,et al.  Age-Specific Preferences for Infant-Directed Affective Intent. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[159]  Laura Lakusta,et al.  The Role of Verbal Repetition in the Development of Infant Speech Preferences From 4 to 14 Months of Age. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[160]  L. Singh,et al.  Influences of Infant-Directed Speech on Early Word Recognition. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[161]  Katherine Demuth,et al.  Effects of the acoustic properties of infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[162]  Ilanit Gordon,et al.  Oxytocin and the Development of Parenting in Humans , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[163]  Alejandrina Cristià,et al.  Phonetic enhancement of sibilants in infant-directed speech. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[164]  W. Verhelst,et al.  Tonal synchrony in mother-infant interaction based on harmonic and pentatonic series. , 2010, Infant behavior & development.

[165]  P. Kaplan,et al.  A Privileged Status for Male Infant-Directed Speech in Infants of Depressed Mothers? Role of Father Involvement. , 2010, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[166]  Shimon Edelman,et al.  An empirical generative framework for computational modeling of language acquisition. , 2010, Journal of child language.

[167]  Laurel Fais,et al.  Now you hear it, now you don't: vowel devoicing in Japanese infant-directed speech. , 2010, Journal of child language.

[168]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Universal production patterns and ambient language influences in babbling: a cross-linguistic study of Korean- and English-learning infants. , 2010, Journal of child language.

[169]  James L. Morgan,et al.  Is early word-form processing stress-full? How natural variability supports recognition , 2010, Cognitive Psychology.

[170]  Yana Yunusova,et al.  Lip movement exaggerations during infant-directed speech. , 2010, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[171]  T. Bergeson,et al.  The effects of age and infant hearing status on maternal use of prosodic cues for clause boundaries in speech. , 2011, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[172]  Kazuyuki Shinohara,et al.  Discrimination between mothers’ infant- and adult-directed speech using hidden Markov models , 2011, Neuroscience Research.

[173]  Donna Erickson,et al.  Processing of infant-directed speech by adults , 2011, NeuroImage.

[174]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Computerized home video detection for motherese may help to study impaired interaction between infants who become autistic and their parents , 2011, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.

[175]  P. Kaplan,et al.  An associative learning deficit in 1-year-old infants of depressed mothers: role of depression duration. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.

[176]  Adena Schachner,et al.  Infant-directed speech drives social preferences in 5-month-old infants. , 2011, Developmental psychology.

[177]  M. Gattis,et al.  Infants infer intentions from prosody , 2012 .

[178]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Do Parentese Prosody and Fathers' Involvement in Interacting Facilitate Social Interaction in Infants Who Later Develop Autism? , 2013, PloS one.