Trial-by-trial predictions of subjective time from human brain activity

Abstract Many contemporary models of time perception are based on the notion that our brain houses an internal “clock”, specialized for tracking duration. Here we show that specialized mechanisms are unnecessary, and that human-like duration judgements can be reconstructed from neural responses during sensory processing. Healthy human participants watched naturalistic, silent videos and rated their duration while fMRI was acquired. We constructed a computational model that predicts video durations from salient events in participants’ visual cortex activation. This model reproduced biases in participants’ subjective reports, whereas control models trained on auditory or somatosensory activity did not. Our data reveal that subjective time is inferred from information arising during the perception of our dynamic sensory environment, providing a computational basis for an end-to-end account of time perception.

[1]  Jacob M. Paul,et al.  A Network of Topographic Maps in Human Association Cortex Hierarchically Transforms Visual Timing-Selective Responses , 2020, Current Biology.

[2]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event Perception and Memory. , 2019, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  A. Seth From Unconscious Inference to the Beholder’s Share: Predictive Perception and Human Experience , 2019, European Review.

[4]  Murray Shanahan,et al.  Activity in perceptual classification networks as a basis for human subjective time perception , 2019, Nature Communications.

[5]  Masamichi J. Hayashi,et al.  Representations of time in human frontoparietal cortex , 2018, Communications Biology.

[6]  Li Lu,et al.  Integrating time from experience in the lateral entorhinal cortex , 2018, Nature.

[7]  Dean V. Buonomano,et al.  The Neural Basis of Timing: Distributed Mechanisms for Diverse Functions , 2018, Neuron.

[8]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event boundaries in memory and cognition , 2017, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[9]  Jonathan W. Pillow,et al.  Discovering Event Structure in Continuous Narrative Perception and Memory , 2016, Neuron.

[10]  Christopher. Simons,et al.  Machine learning with Python , 2017 .

[11]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Brain networks for confidence weighting and hierarchical inference during probabilistic learning , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  Joseph J. Paton,et al.  Midbrain dopamine neurons control judgment of time , 2016, Science.

[13]  D. Terhune,et al.  Time dilates after spontaneous blinking , 2016, Current Biology.

[14]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Predictions Shape Confidence in Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Liang Wang,et al.  Probabilistic Maps of Visual Topography in Human Cortex. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Warren H. Meck,et al.  Oscillatory multiplexing of neural population codes for interval timing and working memory , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  Samuel Gershman,et al.  Statistical Computations Underlying the Dynamics of Memory Updating , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[18]  Mark Jenkinson,et al.  The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project , 2013, NeuroImage.

[19]  A. Clark Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[20]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  ImageNet classification with deep convolutional neural networks , 2012, Commun. ACM.

[21]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Prediction Error Associated with the Perceptual Segmentation of Naturalistic Events , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  E. Maris,et al.  Prior Expectation Mediates Neural Adaptation to Repeated Sounds in the Auditory Cortex: An MEG Study , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  T. Kellermann,et al.  Modality-specific perceptual expectations selectively modulate baseline activity in auditory, somatosensory, and visual cortices. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Gaël Varoquaux,et al.  Scikit-learn: Machine Learning in Python , 2011, J. Mach. Learn. Res..

[25]  Jim M. Monti,et al.  Expectation and Surprise Determine Neural Population Responses in the Ventral Visual Stream , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  D. Eagleman Human time perception and its illusions , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[28]  J H Wearden,et al.  Slowing down an Internal Clock: Implications for Accounts of Performance on four Timing Tasks , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[29]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event perception: a mind-brain perspective. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[30]  Emery N Brown,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Activity during the Gradual Acquisition and Expression of Paired-Associate Memory , 2005 .

[31]  W. Meck,et al.  Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[32]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[33]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[34]  M. Treisman,et al.  The Internal Clock: Electroencephalographic Evidence for Oscillatory Processes Underlying Time Perception , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[35]  D. Homa,et al.  Duration judgment and the experience of change , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[36]  M. Treisman Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". , 1963, Psychological monographs.