Relationship of lesion location to cognitive outcome following microelectrode-guided pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease: support for the existence of cognitive circuits in the human pallidum.

Current models of basal ganglia anatomy posit the existence of multiple parallel, anatomically segregated circuits. Anatomical data from non-human primates suggest that the circuits subserving motor functions are segregated from those subserving cognitive functions. Here we present data that demonstrate that, in humans, motor and cognitive frontosubcortical circuits are segregated. We studied a group of patients with Parkinson's disease undergoing surgical lesioning of the globus pallidus internus for relief of their symptoms. Lesion location along an anteromedial-to-posterolateral axis was found to be related to postsurgical outcome on both cognitive and motor measures. Performance on several neuropsychological measures, including the generation of category exemplars and continuous mental addition, was linearly related to distance along this axis, with anteromedial lesions leading to postsurgical impairment, intermediate lesions having little effect and posterolateral lesions leading to an improvement on several measures. The same relationship was found between memory performance under conditions of proactive interference and lesion location within the globus pallidus internus. In contrast, bradykinesia, assessed as the speed of finger-tapping, had a non-linear relationship to lesion location, intermediate lesions leading to greater postsurgical improvement than lesions in more extreme anteromedial or posterolateral locations. These data demonstrate that the cognitive effects of pallidotomy can be dissociated from the motor effects. These effects depend upon the placement of the lesions within the globus pallidus internus, supporting the segregation of functionally distinct circuits in the human pallidum.

[1]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Neuropsychological consequences of posteroventral pallidotomy for the treatment of Parkinson's disease , 1998, Neurology.

[2]  P. Strick,et al.  Multiple output channels in the basal ganglia. , 1993, Science.

[3]  T. Aziz,et al.  Neuropsychological, neurological and functional outcome following pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease. A consecutive series of eight simultaneous bilateral and twelve unilateral procedures. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[4]  A. Lang,et al.  Posteroventral medial pallidotomy in advanced Parkinson's disease. , 1997, Advances in neurology.

[5]  H. Nauta,et al.  Cognitive sequelae of unilateral posteroventral pallidotomy. , 1997, Archives of neurology.

[6]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  R. Turner,et al.  Treatment of advanced Parkinson's disease by posterior GPi pallidotomy: 1‐year results of a pilot study , 1996, Annals of neurology.

[8]  A. Lang,et al.  Utility of an objective dyskinesia rating scale for Parkinson's disease: Inter‐ and intrarater reliability assessment , 1994, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[9]  Y. Agid,et al.  Pallidal stimulation for Parkinson's disease , 1997, Neurology.

[10]  J C Rothwell,et al.  A study of medial pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease: clinical outcome, MRI location and complications. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  K. Flowers,et al.  The effect of Parkinson's disease on the ability to maintain a mental set. , 1985, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[12]  D. Gronwall Paced Auditory Serial-Addition Task: A Measure of Recovery from Concussion , 1977, Perceptual and motor skills.

[13]  A. Benabid,et al.  Opposite motor effects of pallidal stimulation in Parkinson's disease , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[14]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Memory and learning in early Parkinson's disease: Evidence for a “frontal lobe syndrome” , 1990, Brain and Cognition.

[15]  P. Pollak,et al.  The core assessment program for intracerebral transplantation. , 1995, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[16]  J. Dostrovsky,et al.  Methods for microelectrode-guided posteroventral pallidotomy. , 1996, Journal of neurosurgery.

[17]  R E Gross,et al.  Relationship of lesion location to clinical outcome following microelectrode-guided pallidotomy for Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  P. Strick,et al.  Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia involvement in higher cognitive function. , 1994, Science.

[19]  C. Marsden,et al.  The functions of the basal ganglia and the paradox of stereotaxic surgery in Parkinson's disease. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  C. Marsden,et al.  Cognitive function in Parkinson's disease: From description to theory , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[21]  A. Nambu,et al.  The distribution of the globus pallidus neurons with input from various cortical areas in the monkeys , 1993, Brain Research.

[22]  D. Wechsler WAIS-R manual : Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised , 1981 .

[23]  E. A. Berg,et al.  A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. , 1948, The Journal of general psychology.