Teacher, Text, and Experience: A Case of Young Children's Scientific Inquiry

In this descriptive case study, we describe a 10-day program of study of motion down inclined planes during which a class of 21 second graders investigated scientific relations such as mass and speed, speed and momentum, and mass and momentum via both text-based experiences ("second-hand investigations") and hands-on, materials-based experiments ("first-hand investigations"). We observed first-hand and second-hand investigations to work in concert to provide young students with rich opportunities to learn about motion and to express their developing understandings of scientific ideas. We highlight evidence of children in the class engaging in several processes integral to inquiry, namely, (a) using data as evidence, (b) evaluating investigative procedures, and (c) making sense of multiple forms of representations.

[1]  Lawrence B. Flick,et al.  Scientific inquiry and nature of science : implications for teaching, learning, and teacher education , 2004 .

[2]  Kathleen E. Metz Reassessment of Developmental Constraints on Children’s Science Instruction , 1995 .

[3]  Annemarie Sullivan Palincsar,et al.  The Learning Environment as a Site of Science Education Reform. , 1995 .

[4]  M. Orsolini 'Dwarfs Do Not Shoot': An Analysis of Children's Justifications , 1993 .

[5]  Fred Lubben,et al.  Children's ideas about the reliability of experimental data , 1996 .

[6]  Danielle Joan Ford The role of text in supporting and extending first-hand investigations in guided inquiry science. , 1999 .

[7]  Paul W. Richardson The culture of education , 1996 .

[8]  J. Piaget Science of education and the psychology of the child , 1970 .

[9]  L. Schauble,et al.  Building Functional Models: Designing an Elbow , 1997 .

[10]  J. Frederiksen,et al.  Inquiry, Modeling, and Metacognition: Making Science Accessible to All Students , 1998 .

[11]  Annemarie Sullivan Palincsar,et al.  Designing a community of practice: Principles and practices of the GIsML community , 1998 .

[12]  Robin Millar,et al.  Investigating in the school science laboratory: conceptual and procedural knowledge and their influence on performance , 1994 .

[13]  E. V. Zee,et al.  Analysis of a student-generated inquiry discussion , 2000 .

[14]  J. Lagowski National Science Education Standards , 1995 .

[15]  R. Millar,et al.  Language and Conceptual Understanding in Science: a comparison of English and Asian language speaking children , 1988 .

[16]  Richard Gott,et al.  Practical work: its role in the understanding of evidence in science , 1996 .

[17]  Kathleen E. Metz The Knowledge Building Enterprises In Science And Elementary School Science Classrooms , 2006 .

[18]  J. Lemke Talking Science: Language, Learning, and Values , 1990 .

[19]  S. Shapin Laboratory life. The social construction of scientific facts , 1981, Medical History.

[20]  Richard Lehrer,et al.  Similarity of form and substance: modeling material kind , 2001 .

[21]  John J. Clement,et al.  Using Bridging Analogies and Anchoring Institutions to Seal with Students' Preconceptions in Physics , 1993 .

[22]  J. Shea National Science Education Standards , 1995 .

[23]  David E. Penner,et al.  Explaining systems: Investigating middle school students' understanding of emergent phenomena , 2000 .

[24]  J. Mokros,et al.  The impact of microcomputer‐based labs on children's ability to interpret graphs , 1987 .

[25]  Gordon Wells,et al.  Language and the Inquiry-Oriented Curriculum , 1995 .

[26]  Robin Millar,et al.  Alternative frameworks or context-specific reasoning? Children's ideas about the motion of projectiles , 1994 .

[27]  Sharan B. Merriam,et al.  Qualitative research and case study applications in education , 1998 .

[28]  M. Hegarty,et al.  Graphs as aids to knowledge construction: Signaling techniques for guiding the process of graph comprehension. , 1999 .

[29]  Susanna Elizabeth Hapgood Motion in action: A study of second graders' trajectories of experience during guided inquiry science instruction , 2003 .

[30]  Priti Shah,et al.  A Model of the Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Graph Comprehension , 1998 .

[31]  J. Osborne,et al.  The place of argumentation in the pedagogy of school science , 1999 .

[32]  J. Wertsch Voices of the Mind: A Sociocultural Approach to Mediated Action , 1992 .

[33]  G. W. Ford,et al.  The structure of knowledge and the curriculum , 1964 .

[34]  P. Scott,et al.  Constructing Scientific Knowledge in the Classroom , 1994 .

[35]  D. Klahr,et al.  All other things being equal: acquisition and transfer of the control of variables strategy. , 1999, Child development.

[36]  Leona Schauble,et al.  Students' Understanding of the Objectives and Procedures of Experimentation in the Science Classroom , 1995 .

[37]  Ioanna Vekiri An investigation of the role of graphical design and student characteristics in scientific reasoning with weather maps , 2001 .

[38]  Annemarie Sullivan Palincsar,et al.  The interplay of first-hand and second-hand investigations to model and support the development of scientific knowledge and reasoning , 2001 .

[39]  K. McGilly,et al.  Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice. , 1994 .

[40]  Carol L. Smith,et al.  Using Conceptual Models to Facilitate Conceptual Change: The Case of Weight-Density Differentiation , 1992 .

[41]  C. Peterson,et al.  A naturalistic study of the production of causal connectives by children , 1985, Journal of Child Language.

[42]  Joseph J. Schwab Problems, Topics, and Issues , 1967 .

[43]  Jean Piaget,et al.  The child and reality: Problems of genetic psychology , 1973 .

[44]  J. Piaget Child's Conception of Movement and Speed , 1970 .

[45]  L. Bloom,et al.  What, when, and how about why: a longitudinal study of early expressions of causality. , 1979, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[46]  M. Donaldson Contextual influences on children's spoken and written explanations , 1996, Applied Psycholinguistics.