Old World monkeys are more similar to humans than New World monkeys when playing a coordination game

There is much debate about how humans' decision-making compares with that of other primates. One way to explore this is to compare species' performance using identical methodologies in games with strategical interactions. We presented a computerized Assurance Game, which was either functionally simultaneous or sequential, to investigate how humans, rhesus monkeys and capuchin monkeys used information in decision-making. All species coordinated via sequential play on the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium, indicating that information about the partner's choice improved decisions. Furthermore, some humans and rhesus monkeys found the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium in the simultaneous game, even when it was the first condition presented. Thus, Old World primates solved the task without any external cues to their partner's choice. Finally, when not explicitly prohibited, humans spontaneously used language to coordinate on the payoff-dominant Nash equilibrium, indicating an alternative mechanism for converting a simultaneous move game into a sequential move game. This phylogenetic distribution implies that no single mechanism drives coordination decisions across the primates, while humans' ability to spontaneously use language to change the structure of the game emphasizes that multiple mechanisms may be used even within the same species. These results provide insight into the evolution of decision-making strategies across the primates.

[1]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2011, Cognition.

[2]  Colin Camerer Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strategic Interaction , 2003 .

[3]  R. Hampton Rhesus monkeys know when they remember , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  T. Insel,et al.  The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. , 1999, Journal of human evolution.

[5]  F. D. Waal,et al.  Payment for labour in monkeys , 2000, Nature.

[6]  Performance in a computerized self-control task by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): The combined influence of effort and delay. , 2007, Learning and motivation.

[7]  R. Miller,et al.  Spatial contiguity of cue, reward, and response in discrimination learning by children. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  J. Brosig Identifying cooperative behavior: some experimental results in a prisoner's dilemma game , 2002 .

[9]  Robert Forsythe,et al.  Selection Criteria in Coordination Games: Some Experimental Results , 1987 .

[10]  B. Skyrms The Stag Hunt and the Evolution of Social Structure , 2003 .

[11]  Alvin E. Roth,et al.  Relative versus Absolute Speed of Adjustment in Strategic Environments: Responder Behavior in Ultimatum Games , 2003 .

[12]  V. Smith Microeconomic Systems as an Experimental Science , 1982 .

[13]  J. Huyck,et al.  Tacit Coordination Games, Strategic Uncertainty, and Coordination Failure , 1990 .

[14]  Michael J. Beran,et al.  Monkeys (Macaca Mulatta and Cebus Apella) and Human Adults and Children (Homo Sapiens) Compare Subsets of Moving Stimuli Based on Numerosity , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[15]  Timothy M. Flemming,et al.  Ordinal judgments of symbolic stimuli by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the effects of differential and nondifferential reward. , 2008, Journal of comparative psychology.

[16]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Rhesus Monkeys Attribute Perceptions to Others , 2005, Current Biology.

[17]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Chimpanzees Are Rational Maximizers in an Ultimatum Game , 2007, Science.

[18]  Derek C. Penn,et al.  On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’ , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  H. Harlow,et al.  The effect of spatial contiguity on discrimination learning by rhesus monkeys. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[20]  D A Washburn,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), video tasks, and implications for stimulus-response spatial contiguity. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[21]  Lisa A Heimbauer,et al.  Comparative Economics: Responses to the Assurance Game in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans Using Equivalent Procedures , 2011 .

[22]  J. D. Smith,et al.  The psychological organization of "uncertainty" responses and "middle" responses: a dissociation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[23]  D. Washburn,et al.  Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Maintain Learning Set Despite Second-Order Stimulus-Response Spatial Discontiguity , 2007 .

[24]  F. Waal,et al.  Capuchins do cooperate: the advantage of an intuitive task , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[25]  A. Silberberg,et al.  Rational maximizing by humans (Homo sapiens) in an ultimatum game , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[26]  T. Groothuis,et al.  Not only states but traits — Humans can identify permanent altruistic dispositions in 20 s , 2010 .

[27]  Timothy M. Flemming,et al.  Analogical reasoning and the differential outcome effect: transitory bridging of the conceptual gap for rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[28]  A. M. Schrier,et al.  Discrimination learning by monkeys with spatial separation of cue and response. , 1962, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[29]  W W CUMMING,et al.  Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. , 1961, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[30]  Analogical Reasoning and Easy Rescue Cases , 1993 .

[31]  D. Washburn,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), video tasks, and implications for stimulus-response spatial contiguity. , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.