Is Inhibition of Return due to attentional disengagement or to a detection cost? The Detection Cost Theory of IOR

When the time interval between two peripheral stimuli is long enough, reaction times (RTs) to targets presented at previously stimulated locations are longer than RTs to targets presented at new locations. This effect is widely known as Inhibition of Return (IOR). The effect is usually explained as an inhibitory bias against returning attention to previously attended locations. Thus, attentional disengagement is considered to be a necessary condition to observe IOR (Klein, 2000). We report data from three experiments with 2 different paradigms in which we show that IOR can be dissociated from the endogenous disengagement of spatial attention. Two main results are reported: 1) IOR is observed at an endogenously attended location in some situations, and 2) even after the endogenous disengagement of attention, facilitation instead of IOR is observed in other situations. We conclude that the endogenous disengagement of attention is neither sufficient nor necessary for IOR to be observed. However, by presenting an intervening event between the cue and the target, an IOR effect occurred in all conditions, indicating the importance of attentional segregation processes (exogenous disengagement) for generating IOR. These results are interpreted on the basis of cue-target event integration and segregation processes (Lupianez, 2010), which constitute our dynamical perceptual experience. IOR is explained as a cost in detecting the appearance of new attention-capturing information (i.e., the target) at locations where attention has been already captured by previous events (the cue).

[1]  B. Milliken,et al.  Subjective expectancy and inhibition of return: A dissociation in a non-spatial two-alternative forced choice task , 2013 .

[2]  R. Klein On the Control of Visual Orienting , 2004 .

[3]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  A. Henik,et al.  Attentional asymmetry in schizophrenia: disengagement and inhibition of return deficits. , 2001, Neuropsychology.

[5]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Dissociating inhibition of return from endogenous orienting of spatial attention: Evidence from detection and discrimination tasks , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[6]  B. Milliken,et al.  The manifestation of attentional capture: facilitation or IOR depending on task demands , 2007, Psychological research.

[7]  James M. Hillis,et al.  The effect of the physical characteristics of cues and targets on facilitation and inhibition , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Timothy J. Wood,et al.  Negative priming without ignoring , 1998 .

[9]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[10]  Qi Chen,et al.  Cross-modal nonspatial repetition inhibition , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[11]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  The role of spatial attention and other processes on the magnitude and time course of cueing effects , 2005, Cognitive Processing.

[12]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Does IOR occur in discrimination tasks? Yes, it does, but later , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[13]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Cue and target processing modulate the onset of inhibition of return. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .

[15]  P. Bartolomeo L EVELS OF IMPAIRMENT IN UNILATERAL NEGLECT , 2001 .

[16]  B. Milliken,et al.  The modulation of exogenous Spatial Cueing on Spatial Stroop interference: Evidence of a set for "cue-target event segregation" , 2008 .

[17]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Reorienting attention and inhibition of return , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Jay Pratt,et al.  Examining the role of the fixation cue in inhibition of return. , 2002, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[19]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Inhibición de retorno sin retorno de la atención , 2004 .

[20]  R. Rafal,et al.  Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[21]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[22]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Is “Inhibition of Return” due to the inhibition of the return of attention? , 2013, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Task dependent modulation of exogenous attention: Effects of target duration and intervening events , 2013, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[24]  G. Tassinari,et al.  Consequences of covert orienting to non-informative stimuli of different modalities: A unitary mechanism? , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  A new estimation of the duration of attentional dwell time , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[27]  B. Milliken,et al.  Separate mechanisms recruited by exogenous and endogenous spatial cues: evidence from a spatial Stroop paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  B. Milliken,et al.  Independent effects of endogenous and exogenous spatial cueing: inhibition of return at endogenously attended target locations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  Chris Moore,et al.  Inhibition of return in children and adolescents. , 2003, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  M. Posner The Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention , 2020 .

[33]  Giovanni Berlucchi,et al.  Inhibition of return: A phenomenon in search of a mechanism and a better name , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  B. Milliken,et al.  Inhibition of return and the attentional set for integrating versus differentiating information. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.

[35]  A. Henik,et al.  The Endogenous Modulation of IOR is Nasal-Temporal Asymmetric , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  B. Milliken,et al.  Orienting in space and time: Joint contributions to exogenous spatial cuing effects , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[39]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  The effects of practice on cueing in detection and discrimination tasks , 2001 .

[40]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  L. Riggio,et al.  The relationship between central cues and peripheral cues in covert visual orientation , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  How object-specific are object files? Evidence for integration by location. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  R A Abrams,et al.  Inhibition of return in discrimination tasks. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  Charles Spence,et al.  Comparing intramodal and crossmodal cuing in the endogenous orienting of spatial attention , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  D. Balota,et al.  Inhibition of return and visuospatial attention in healthy older adults and individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[46]  John D. Bransford,et al.  Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing , 1977 .

[47]  J. Juola,et al.  Voluntary allocation versus automatic capture of visual attention , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[48]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return: Twenty years after , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[50]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. , 2002 .

[51]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Do peripheral non-informative cues induce early facilitation of target detection? , 1994, Vision Research.

[52]  B. Milliken,et al.  Response to an intervening event reverses nonspatial repetition effects in 2AFC tasks: Nonspatial IOR? , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[53]  G. Berlucchi,et al.  Reaction time inhibition from subliminal cues: Is it related to inhibition of return? , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Kristie R. Dukewich Reconceptualizing inhibition of return as , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[57]  M. Corbetta,et al.  An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Volitional Covert Orienting to a Peripheral Cue Does Not Suppress Cue-induced Inhibition of Return , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  W. Neill,et al.  Does “inhibition of return” occur in discrimination tasks? , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Unmasking the inhibition of return phenomenon , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[61]  S. Tipper,et al.  Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[62]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[63]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Spatial distribution of the inhibitory effect of peripheral non-informative cues on simple reaction time to non-fixated visual targets , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  S. Tipper,et al.  On the Strategic Modulation of the Time Course of Facilitation and Inhibition of Return , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[65]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Covert attention affects the psychometric function of contrast sensitivity , 2002, Vision Research.

[66]  A. Samuel,et al.  Eliminating inhibition of return by changing salient nonspatial attributes in a complex environment. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[67]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Effects of endogenous and exogenous attention on visual processing: An Inhibition of Return study , 2009, Brain Research.

[68]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .