Investigating the Role of Response in Spatial Context Learning
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B. Scholl,et al. The Automaticity of Visual Statistical Learning Statistical Learning , 2005 .
[2] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual , 1999, Memory & cognition.
[3] M. Ziessler,et al. Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: further evidence from a serial reaction task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[4] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[5] L. Barsalou. Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.
[6] Zhuanghua Shi,et al. Contextual cueing in multiconjunction visual search is dependent on color- and configuration-based intertrial contingencies. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[8] A. Allport,et al. Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .
[9] M. Chun,et al. Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[10] Tal Makovski,et al. Visual Learning in Multiple-Object Tracking , 2008, PloS one.
[11] Marcel Brass,et al. The cognitive representation of intending not to act: Evidence for specific non-action-effect binding , 2010, Cognition.
[12] A. Kramer,et al. Attentional guidance of the eyes by contextual information and abrupt onsets. , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[14] Armin Stock,et al. A short history of ideo-motor action , 2004, Psychological research.
[15] F. Strack,et al. Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: a case of conceptual-motor compatibility. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[16] A. Reber. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge , 1993 .
[17] M. Chun,et al. Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .
[18] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Phasic Activation of Monkey Locus Ceruleus Neurons by Simple Decisions in a Forced-Choice Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[19] M. Chun,et al. Perceptual constraints on implicit learning of spatial context , 2002 .
[20] Yuhong V. Jiang,et al. The Attentional Boost Effect: Transient increases in attention to one task enhance performance in a second task , 2010, Cognition.
[21] G. Aston-Jones,et al. Locus coeruleus neurons in monkey are selectively activated by attended cues in a vigilance task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[22] M. Chun,et al. Implicit, long-term spatial contextual memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[23] John R. Duncan,et al. Visual search and visual attention , 1985 .
[24] Philip David Zelazo,et al. Evidence for intact memory-guided attention in school-aged children. , 2010, Developmental science.
[25] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.
[26] K. Nakayama,et al. Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] C. Frith,et al. Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] Melvyn A. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action (precis) , 1998 .
[29] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[30] B. Hommel,et al. Acquisition and generalization of action effects , 2003 .
[31] Yuhong Jiang,et al. Attending to unrelated targets boosts short-term memory for color arrays , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[32] Colin M. Macleod,et al. The production effect: delineation of a phenomenon. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[33] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Sequences learned without awareness can orient attention during the perception of human activity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[34] J. H. Howard,et al. Developmental differences in implicit learning of spatial context. , 2007, Neuropsychology.
[35] Edward Awh,et al. Statistical learning induces discrete shifts in the allocation of working memory resources. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] B. Hommel,et al. Effect anticipation and action control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] K. Nakayama,et al. Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention , 1989, Vision Research.
[38] Marvin M Chun,et al. Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[39] M. Chun,et al. Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.
[40] Yuhong V Jiang,et al. The role of timing in the attentional boost effect , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[41] Jeremy M Wolfe,et al. Visual Attention , 2020, Computational Models for Cognitive Vision.
[42] Timothy F. Brady,et al. Spatial constraints on learning in visual search: modeling contextual cuing. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[43] S. Hahn,et al. The effect of behavioral response on affective evaluation. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[44] J Miller,et al. Components of the location probability effect in visual search tasks. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[46] Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al. Does contextual cueing guide the deployment of attention , 2010 .
[47] Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al. Event boundaries in perception affect memory encoding and updating. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[48] G. Remillard. Pure perceptual-based sequence learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[49] Joo-Hyun Song,et al. Spatial context learning in visual search and change detection , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] David J. Heeger,et al. Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[51] James H. Howard,et al. Implicit sequence learning without motor sequencing in young and old adults , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] Yuhong V. Jiang,et al. What is learned in spatial contextual cuing— configuration or individual locations? , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[53] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[54] R. Aslin,et al. Statistical learning of higher-order temporal structure from visual shape sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[55] Yuhong V Jiang,et al. Interrupted visual searches reveal volatile search memory. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Melina A. Kunar,et al. Does contextual cuing guide the deployment of attention? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[58] Christopher C. Davoli,et al. Altered vision near the hands , 2008, Cognition.