Teach to reach: The effects of active vs. passive reaching experiences on action and perception

Reaching is an important and early emerging motor skill that allows infants to interact with the physical and social world. However, few studies have considered how reaching experiences shape infants' own motor development and their perception of actions performed by others. In the current study, two groups of infants received daily parent guided play sessions over a 2-weeks training period. Using "Sticky Mittens", one group was enabled to independently pick up objects whereas the other group only passively observed their parent's actions on objects. Following training, infants' manual and visual exploration of objects, agents, and actions in a live and a televised context were assessed. Our results showed that only infants who experienced independent object apprehension advanced in their reaching behavior, and showed changes in their visual exploration of agents and objects in a live setting. Passive observation was not sufficient to change infants' behavior. To our surprise, the effects of the training did not seem to generalize to a televised observation context. Together, our results suggest that early motor training can jump-start infants' transition into reaching and inform their perception of others' actions.

[1]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  Early Experience and Emotional Development: The Emergence of Wariness of Heights , 1992 .

[2]  M. Onis WHO Motor Development Study: Windows of achievement for six gross motor development milestones , 2006, Acta paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992). Supplement.

[3]  K. Adolph,et al.  Learning in the development of infant locomotion. , 1997, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[4]  D. Gentner,et al.  Structural alignment facilitates the noticing of differences , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[5]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.

[6]  M. Appelbaum,et al.  Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its timing. , 1995, Child development.

[7]  Melissa W Clearfield,et al.  Learning by looking: Infants' social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[8]  Carolyn Rovee-Collier,et al.  The Development of Infant Memory , 1999 .

[9]  Gisa Aschersleben,et al.  Infants' perception of goal-directed actions on video , 2007 .

[10]  A. Decasper,et al.  Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates , 1981 .

[11]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[12]  J. Brown,et al.  Posture as a determinant of visual behavior in newborns. , 1975, Child development.

[13]  W. Eaton,et al.  Sex differences in human motor activity level. , 1986, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[15]  J. Javůrek [Motor development]. , 1988, Ceskoslovenska pediatrie.

[16]  D. Kuhn,et al.  Cognition, Perception, and Language , 1997 .

[17]  Lisa Riolo,et al.  Three Month Old Infants Can Learn Task Specific Patterns of Interlimb Coordination , 1995 .

[18]  T. Bower,et al.  Demonstration of Intention in the Reaching Behaviour of Neonate Humans , 1970, Nature.

[19]  D. Gentner,et al.  Structure mapping in the comparison process. , 2000, The American journal of psychology.

[20]  Janny C. Stapel,et al.  Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[21]  K. Adolph Specificity of Learning: Why Infants Fall Over a Veritable Cliff , 2000, Psychological science.

[22]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Baby do – Baby see! How action production influences action perception in infants , 2007 .

[23]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy , 2005, Cognition.

[24]  J. Deloache,et al.  The medium can obscure the message: young children's understanding of video. , 1998, Child development.

[25]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Travel Broadens the Mind. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[26]  M. Clearfield Learning to walk changes infants' social interactions. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.

[27]  J. Piaget,et al.  The origin of intelligence in the child , 1997 .

[28]  Marion A. Eppler,et al.  Development of manipulatory skills and the deployment of attention , 1995 .

[29]  C. Hofsten Eye–hand coordination in the newborn. , 1982 .

[30]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[32]  A. Needham Improvements in Object Exploration Skills May Facilitate the Development of Object Segregation in Early Infancy , 2000 .

[33]  E. Gibson Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. , 1988 .

[34]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  An Epigenetic Perspective on the Development of Self-Produced Locomotion and Its Consequences , 1994 .

[35]  A. Needham,et al.  Embodiment in early development. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[36]  A. Pomerleau,et al.  Development of cardiac and behavioral responses to a three-dimensional toy stimulation in one- to six-month-old infants. , 1980, Child development.

[37]  Kazuo Hiraki,et al.  Infant's brain responses to live and televised action , 2006, NeuroImage.

[38]  I. Sigel,et al.  HANDBOOK OF CHILD PSYCHOLOGY , 2006 .

[39]  R. Held,et al.  MOVEMENT-PRODUCED STIMULATION IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY GUIDED BEHAVIOR. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[40]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Haptic perception: A tutorial , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[41]  Emese Nagy,et al.  Gender-Related Differences in Neonatal Imitation. , 2007 .

[42]  C. K. Rovee,et al.  Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. , 1969, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[43]  E. Bushnell,et al.  Motor development and the mind: the potential role of motor abilities as a determinant of aspects of perceptual development. , 1993, Child development.

[44]  D. Messinger,et al.  Posture and gaze in early mother–infant communication: synchronization of developmental trajectories , 1999 .

[45]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[46]  E. Gibson,et al.  The "visual cliff". , 1960, Scientific American.

[47]  P. Hauf Infants' perception and production of intentional actions. , 2007, Progress in brain research.

[48]  Michael Gasser,et al.  The Development of Embodied Cognition: Six Lessons from Babies , 2005, Artificial Life.

[49]  Hein T. van Schie,et al.  You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.

[50]  A. Needham,et al.  A pick-me-up for infants’ exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using ‘sticky mittens’ enhances young infants’ object exploration skills , 2002 .

[51]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.