The benefit of no choice: goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
Markus Janczyk | Peter M. Gollwitzer | Michael Dambacher | Maik Bieleke | P. Gollwitzer | M. Dambacher | M. Bieleke | M. Janczyk
[1] J. Hoffmann,et al. Learning at any rate: action–effect learning for stimulus-based actions , 2011, Psychological research.
[2] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[4] Arvid Herwig,et al. Action–effect associations revealed by eye movements , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Die Reaktion als Willenshandlung , 1998 .
[6] B. Hommel,et al. Effect anticipation and action control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[8] M. Brass,et al. The What, When, Whether Model of Intentional Action , 2008, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[9] C. Midden,et al. To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviors , 1999 .
[10] P. Sheeran,et al. How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes , 2007 .
[11] Sam J. Maglio,et al. Action control by implementation intentions : The role of discrete emotions , 2013 .
[12] Andrea Kiesel,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Do Endogenous and Exogenous Action Control Compete for Perception ? , 2011 .
[13] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[14] P. Gollwitzer,et al. Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .
[15] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Effective Rotations: Action Effects Determine the Interplay of Mental and Manual Rotations Interactions of Manual and Mental Rotations , 2022 .
[16] C. Frith. The psychology of volition , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. No differences in dual-task costs between forced- and free-choice tasks , 2014, Psychological Research.
[18] P. Gollwitzer,et al. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes , 2006 .
[19] P. Sheeran,et al. Motivational and volitional processes in action initiation: A field study of the role of implementation intentions , 2000 .
[20] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Visual and tactile action effects determine bimanual coordination performance. , 2009, Human movement science.
[21] Markus Janczyk,et al. Level 2 perspective taking entails two processes: evidence from PRP experiments. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[23] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[25] P. Gollwitzer. Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. , 1999 .
[26] P. Gollwitzer. Goal Achievement: The Role of Intentions , 1993 .
[27] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[28] Saul Sternberg,et al. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .
[29] C. Agnew. Then a miracle occurs : focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research : Purdue Symposium on Psychological Sciences , 2010 .
[30] Anja Achtziger,et al. Committing to implementation intentions: Attention and memory effects for selected situational cues , 2012 .
[31] C. Kennard,et al. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[32] Ann Reynolds,et al. The locus of redundant-targets and nontargets effects: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] A. Schubö,et al. Action Intentions Modulate Allocation of Visual Attention: Electrophysiological Evidence , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[34] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[35] W. Kunde,et al. Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[36] Patrick Haggard,et al. What are self-generated actions? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.
[37] K. Sassenberg,et al. I can’t take my eyes off of it : Attention attraction effects of implementation intentions , 2006 .
[38] R. Gaschler,et al. Instructed Task Demands and Utilization of Action Effect Anticipation , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[39] A. Kiesel,et al. Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[40] Wilfried Kunde,et al. The locus of tool-transformation costs. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] W. G. Koster,et al. The psychological refractory period , 1966 .
[42] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[43] P. Jolicoeur,et al. Decision and Response in Dual-Task Interference , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.
[44] G. Dreisbach,et al. Bidirectional priming processes in the Simon task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] Richard Schweickert,et al. A critical path generalization of the additive factor method: Analysis of a stroop task , 1978 .
[46] R. Pfister,et al. Instant Attraction: Immediate Action-Effect Bindings Occur for Both, Stimulus- and Goal-Driven Actions , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[47] Sean M. McCrea,et al. How to Maximize Implementation Intention Effects , 2009 .
[48] D. Berlyne. Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts. , 1957, Psychological review.
[49] Uwe Mattler,et al. Time course of free-choice priming effects explained by a simple accumulator model , 2012, Cognition.
[50] P. Gollwitzer,et al. Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[51] S. Dehaene,et al. Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.
[52] B. Hommel,et al. Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[53] D. Berlyne. Conflict and choice time. , 1957, British journal of psychology.
[54] Gabriele Oettingen,et al. Action control by implementation intentions : effective cue detection and efficient response initiation , 2007 .
[55] W. Kunde,et al. Dissecting the response in response–effect compatibility , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.
[57] Arvid Herwig,et al. Action-Effect Bindings and Ideomotor Learning in Intention- and Stimulus-Based Actions , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[58] M. Steinhauser,et al. A dual-stage two-phase model of selective attention. , 2010, Psychological review.
[59] P. Gollwitzer,et al. Reflective and reflexive action control in patients with frontal brain lesions. , 2001, Neuropsychology.
[60] Anja Achtziger,et al. Responding to subliminal cues: Do if-then plans facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious intent? , 2009 .
[61] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Busy doing nothing: Evidence for nonaction-effect binding , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[62] Arvid Herwig,et al. Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[63] R. Passingham,et al. Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one's own performance , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[64] Hannes Ruge,et al. On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[65] M. Posner,et al. Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.
[66] Wolfgang Prinz,et al. Two Modes of Sensorimotor Integration in Intention-Based and Stimulus-Based Actions , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[67] R. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and psychological refractory period effects: Implications for response selection , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[68] Agnieszka Wykowska,et al. How you move is what you see: action planning biases selection in visual search. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.