Perception of structure from motion: is projective correspondence of moving elements a necessary condition?

A fundamental assumption of almost all existing computational analyses of the perception of structure from motion is that moving elements on the retina projectively correspond to identifiable moving points in three-dimensional space. The present investigation was designed to determine the psychological validity of this assumption in several different contexts. The results demonstrate that the ability of human observers to perceive structure from motion is much more general than would be reasonable to expect on the basis of existing theory. Observers can experience a compelling kinetic depth effect even when the pattern of optical motion is contaminated by large amounts of visual noise (e.g., where the signal to noise ratio is less than 0.15). Moreover, the optical deformations of shading, texture, or self-occluding contours, which would be treated as noise by existing computational models, are analyzed by human observers as perceptually salient sources of information about an object's three-dimensional form. These results suggest that the modular analyses of visual information that currently dominate the literature will have to be modified if they are to account for the high level of generality exhibited by human observers.

[1]  E. Mingolla,et al.  Perception of surface curvature and direction of illumination from patterns of shading. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J T Todd,et al.  Visual Perception of Relative Mass in Dynamic Events , 1982, Perception.

[3]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985, Psychological review.

[4]  Andrea J. van Doorn,et al.  Invariant Properties of the Motion Parallax Field due to the Movement of Rigid Bodies Relative to an Observer , 1975 .

[5]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985 .

[6]  Myron L. Braunstein,et al.  Depth perception through motion , 1976 .

[7]  J. Gibson,et al.  Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  J. Koenderink,et al.  The Shape of Smooth Objects and the Way Contours End , 1982, Perception.

[9]  S. Ullman The Interpretation of Visual Motion , 1979 .

[10]  S. Grossberg The quantized geometry of visual space: The coherent computation of depth, form, and lightness , 1982, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[11]  H. Wallach,et al.  The kinetic depth effect. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[13]  J. Todd Visual information about rigid and nonrigid motion: a geometric analysis. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  J. Cutting,et al.  Three gradients and the perception of flat and curved surfaces. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[15]  J T Todd,et al.  Simulation of curved surfaces from patterns of optical texture. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[17]  H. Flock SOME CONDITIONS SUFFICIENT FOR ACCURATE MONOCULAR PERCEPTIONS OF MOVING SURFACE SLANTS. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  Jaakko Hintikka,et al.  On the Logic of Perception , 1969 .

[19]  H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al.  The interpretation of a moving retinal image , 1980, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[20]  J S Lappin,et al.  Accurate visual measurement of three-dimensional moving patterns. , 1983, Science.

[21]  Ellen C. Hildreth,et al.  Measurement of Visual Motion , 1984 .

[22]  J T Petersik,et al.  Three-dimensional object constancy: Coherence of a simulated rotating sphere in noise , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  J T Todd,et al.  The perception of three-dimensional structure from rigid and nonrigid motion , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  James T. Todd,et al.  Computational techniques for the graphic simulation of quadric surfaces , 1984 .

[25]  J. Gibson,et al.  Continuous perspective transformations and the perception of rigid motion. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[27]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Detectability of velocity gradients in moving random-dot patterns , 1983, Vision Research.

[28]  J. Koenderink,et al.  Photometric Invariants Related to Solid Shape , 1980 .

[29]  H. Flock,et al.  Variables of Surface Texture and Accuracy of Space Perceptions , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[30]  M. Braunstein Contrasts between Human and Machine Vision: Should Technology Recapitulate Phylogeny? , 1983 .

[31]  G J Andersen,et al.  Shape and depth perception from parallel projections of three-dimensional motion. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  J T Todd,et al.  Visual information about moving objects. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  J S Lappin,et al.  Detection of three-dimensional structure in moving optical patterns. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  F. Restle Coding theory of the perception of motion configurations. , 1979, Psychological review.

[35]  Berthold K. P. Horn,et al.  Determining Optical Flow , 1981, Other Conferences.

[36]  Adolfo Guzmán-Arenas,et al.  COMPUTER RECOGNITION OF THREE-DIMENSIONAL OBJECTS IN A VISUAL SCENE , 1968 .

[37]  Azriel Rosenfeld,et al.  Human and Machine Vision , 1983 .

[38]  A. Ames Visual perception and the rotating trapezoidal window , 1951 .

[39]  J. J. Koenderink,et al.  Temporal properties of the visual detectability of moving spatial white noise , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  J S Lappin,et al.  Minimal conditions for the visual detection of structure and motion in three dimensions. , 1980, Science.