Visual intensity-dependent response latencies predict perceived audio–visual simultaneity

Abstract To form a coherent presentation of the world, the brain needs to combine multiple sensory modalities accurately together in the temporal domain. Judgements on the relative timing of audio–visual stimuli are complex, due to the differing propagation speeds of light and sound through the environment and the nervous system, and the dependence of processing latencies on stimulus intensity (Pieron, 1913). Simultaneity judgement (SJ) and temporal order judgement (TOJ) tasks are often used to assess the temporal mechanisms underlying this binding process. However, these tasks consistently produce measures of perceived simultaneity that are uncorrelated with each other, leading to the suggestion that SJ and TOJ tasks could depend on separate neural mechanisms. Parise and Ernst’s (2016) multisensory correlation detector (MCD) model predicts this lack of correlation by assuming two internal processing stages, a lag computation and a correlation. Here we include and empirically evaluate an intensity-dependent processing delay in the MCD model. We estimate the points of subjective simultaneity (PSSs) using both SJ and TOJ tasks for four different visual intensities and a fixed auditory sound level. Evaluation of four variants of the intensity-dependent MCD model shows that the introduction of an early processing delay can predict the different PSS values obtained in the two respective tasks, without the need for later intensity-dependent multisensorial processing stages. Crucially, this early processing delay can be estimated from simple reaction times.

[1]  J Miller,et al.  Effects of stimulus duration and intensity on simple reaction time and response force. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Zachary P. Barnett,et al.  Interactions between the spatial and temporal stimulus factors that influence multisensory integration in human performance , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  M. Binder Neural correlates of audiovisual temporal processing – Comparison of temporal order and simultaneity judgments , 2015, Neuroscience.

[4]  Simon Carlile,et al.  Synchronizing to real events: subjective audiovisual alignment scales with perceived auditory depth and speed of sound. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  C. Spence,et al.  Confusing the mind by crossing the hands. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  W Reichardt,et al.  Functional structure of a mechanism of perception of optical movement , 1958 .

[7]  Virginie van Wassenhove,et al.  Dissociating the sequential dependency of subjective temporal order from subjective simultaneity , 2019, PloS one.

[8]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Temporal mechanisms of multimodal binding , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Avoiding pitfalls when using information-theoretic methods , 2002 .

[10]  M. Symonds,et al.  A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike’s information criterion , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[11]  F. Lin,et al.  Onset timing of cross‐sensory activations and multisensory interactions in auditory and visual sensory cortices , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  R. Ulrich Threshold models of temporal-order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  Marc O. Ernst,et al.  Correlation detection as a general mechanism for multisensory integration , 2016, Nature Communications.

[15]  F. Pollick,et al.  A Psychophysical Investigation of Differences between Synchrony and Temporal Order Judgments , 2013, PloS one.

[16]  Mark E. McCourt,et al.  The question of simultaneity in multisensory integration , 2012, Electronic Imaging.

[17]  M. Wallace,et al.  Event Related Potentials Index Rapid Recalibration to Audiovisual Temporal Asynchrony , 2017, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[18]  Sophie M. Wuerger,et al.  Reaction time facilitation for horizontally moving auditory-visual stimuli. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[19]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Auditory facilitation of visual-target detection persists regardless of retinal eccentricity and despite wide audiovisual misalignments , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  M. McCourt,et al.  The roles of physical and physiological simultaneity in audiovisual multisensory facilitation , 2013, i-Perception.

[21]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Timecourse of coactivation in bimodal divided attention , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  C. Spence,et al.  Audio-visual simultaneity judgments , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  B. Stein,et al.  Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  Joel Pokorny,et al.  Linking impulse response functions to reaction time: Rod and cone reaction time data and a computational model , 2007, Vision Research.

[25]  M. García-Pérez,et al.  On the discrepant results in synchrony judgment and temporal-order judgment tasks: a quantitative model , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  C. Spence,et al.  Audiovisual temporal adaptation of speech: temporal order versus simultaneity judgments , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  C. Spence,et al.  Audiovisual temporal order judgments , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  H. Piéron,et al.  II. Recherches sur les lois de variation des temps de latence sensorielle en fonction des intensités excitatrices , 1913 .

[29]  L. Harris,et al.  Perceived timing of vestibular stimulation relative to touch, light and sound , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  M. McCourt,et al.  Dissociation of perception and action in audiovisual multisensory integration , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  M. Carrillo-de-la-Peña,et al.  The effects of stimulus intensity and age on visual-evoked potentials (VEPs) in normal children. , 1999, Psychophysiology.

[32]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Latency functions in human vision. , 1973, Vision research.

[33]  P. Jaśkowski Two-stage model for order discrimination , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Do People Take Stimulus Correlations into Account in Visual Search? , 2016, PloS one.

[35]  N. Bolognini,et al.  “Acoustical vision” of below threshold stimuli: interaction among spatially converging audiovisual inputs , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  A. Holcombe,et al.  Differences in perceptual latency estimated from judgments of temporal order, simultaneity and duration are inconsistent , 2014, i-Perception.

[37]  M. Wallace,et al.  The construct of the multisensory temporal binding window and its dysregulation in developmental disabilities , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  M. Wallace,et al.  Individual differences in the multisensory temporal binding window predict susceptibility to audiovisual illusions. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  D. Pins,et al.  On the relation between stimulus intensity and processing time: Piéron’s law and choice reaction time , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[40]  Piotr Jaśkowski Temporal-order judgment and reaction time for short and long stimuli , 1992, Psychological research.

[41]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  A. J. King,et al.  Integration of visual and auditory information in bimodal neurones in the guinea-pig superior colliculus , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  Matthew A. De Niear,et al.  Audiovisual Simultaneity Judgment and Rapid Recalibration throughout the Lifespan , 2016, PloS one.

[44]  A. Gorea,et al.  Temporal order judgment and simple reaction times: evidence for a common processing system. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[45]  M. Miyazaki,et al.  Dissociating the neural correlates of tactile temporal order and simultaneity judgements , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[46]  Lars T. Boenke,et al.  Stimulus duration influences perceived simultaneity in audiovisual temporal-order judgment , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Divided attention: Evidence for coactivation with redundant signals , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  J. Juola,et al.  Audiovisual synchrony and temporal order judgments: Effects of experimental method and stimulus type , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  F. Jaroszyk,et al.  The effect of stimulus intensity on force output in simple reaction time task in humans. , 1995, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[50]  Darren E. Koenig,et al.  The absolute threshold of cone vision. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[51]  D. Alais,et al.  Rapid Recalibration to Audiovisual Asynchrony , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  A. Diederich,et al.  The time window of multisensory integration: relating reaction times and judgments of temporal order. , 2015, Psychological review.

[53]  Albert R. Powers,et al.  Perceptual Training Narrows the Temporal Window of Multisensory Binding , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.