Kin rejection: social signals, neural response and perceived distress during social exclusion.

Across species, kin bond together to promote survival. We sought to understand the dyadic effect of exclusion by kin (as opposed to non-kin strangers) on brain activity of the mother and her child and their subjective distress. To this end, we probed mother-child relationships with a computerized ball-toss game Cyberball. When excluded by one another, rather than by a stranger, both mothers and children exhibited a significantly pronounced frontal P2. Moreover, upon kin rejection versus stranger rejection, both mothers and children showed incremented left frontal positive slow waves for rejection events. Children reported more distress upon exclusion than their own mothers. Similar to past work, relatively augmented negative frontal slow wave activity predicted greater self-reported ostracism distress. This effect, generalized to the P2, was limited to mother- or child-rejection by kin, with comparable magnitude of effect across kin identity (mothers vs. children). For both mothers and children, the frontal P2 peak was significantly pronounced for kin rejection versus stranger rejection. Taken together, our results document the rapid categorization of social signals as kin relevant and the specificity of early and late neural markers for predicting felt ostracism.

[1]  P Fonagy,et al.  The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. , 1996, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[2]  G. S. Pettit,et al.  Friendships with Peers Who are Low or High in Aggression as Moderators of the Link between Peer Victimization and Declines in Academic Functioning , 2008, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[3]  Carrie L. Masten,et al.  Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: understanding the distress of peer rejection. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[4]  K. Williams,et al.  Cyberball: A program for use in research on interpersonal ostracism and acceptance , 2006, Behavior research methods.

[5]  Michael Murias,et al.  Response to familiar faces, newly familiar faces, and novel faces as assessed by ERPs is intact in adults with autism spectrum disorders. , 2010, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[6]  Melissa A. Kluczynski,et al.  Impact of simulated ostracism on overweight and normal-weight youths’ motivation to eat and food intake , 2011, Appetite.

[7]  D. Benoit,et al.  Stability and transmission of attachment across three generations. , 1994, Child development.

[8]  Erik Asp,et al.  A Quantitative Meta-Analysis of Functional Imaging Studies of Social Rejection , 2013, Scientific Reports.

[9]  L. Lehtonen,et al.  Closeness and separation in neonatal intensive care , 2012, Acta paediatrica.

[10]  Florence Thibaut,et al.  ERPs ASSOCIATED WITH FAMILIARITY AND DEGREE OF FAMILIARITY DURING FACE RECOGNITION , 2002, The International journal of neuroscience.

[11]  R. Sullivan,et al.  Infant bonding and attachment to the caregiver: insights from basic and clinical science. , 2011, Clinics in perinatology.

[12]  R. Isabella Origins of attachment: maternal interactive behavior across the first year. , 1993, Child development.

[13]  K. Williams,et al.  When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[14]  Peter J. Molfese,et al.  Social exclusion in middle childhood: Rejection events, slow-wave neural activity, and ostracism distress , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[15]  M. Sarlo,et al.  When Faces Signal Danger: Event-Related Potentials to Emotional Facial Expressions in Animal Phobics , 2010, Neuropsychobiology.

[16]  G. Klann-Delius,et al.  The Development of Communicative Competence of Securely and Insecurely Attached Children in Interactions with Their Mothers , 1997, Journal of psycholinguistic research.

[17]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: Implications of social neuroscience for education , 2011, NeuroImage.

[18]  M. Bakermans-Kranenburg,et al.  Does intranasal oxytocin promote prosocial behavior to an excluded fellow player? A randomized-controlled trial with Cyberball , 2013, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[19]  B. J. Fidler,et al.  CHILDREN RESISTING POSTSEPARATION CONTACT WITH A PARENT: CONCEPTS, CONTROVERSIES, AND CONUNDRUMS , 2010 .

[20]  Young Shin Kim,et al.  School Bullying and Suicidal Risk in Korean Middle School Students , 2005, Pediatrics.

[21]  Bregtje Gunther Moor,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Social Neuroscience Do You like Me? Neural Correlates of Social Evaluation and Developmental Trajectories Do You like Me? Neural Correlates of Social Evaluation and Developmental Trajectories Neural Correlates of Peer Evaluation , 2022 .

[22]  Jessica L Borelli,et al.  Play it again: neural responses to reunion with excluders predicted by attachment patterns. , 2013, Developmental science.

[23]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[24]  Jessica L Borelli,et al.  Attachment dismissal predicts frontal slow-wave ERPs during rejection by unfamiliar peers. , 2012, Emotion.

[25]  Glenn I. Roisman,et al.  The Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of attachment style: an empirical rapprochement. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  Marinus H. van IJzendoorn,et al.  Sensitivity and Attachment: A Meta‐Analysis on Parental Antecedents of Infant Attachment , 1997 .

[27]  M. Flaten,et al.  Variability in placebo analgesia and the role of fear of pain—an ERP study , 2011, PAIN®.

[28]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Event-related potentials of recognizing happy and neutral faces , 2001, Neuroreport.

[29]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion , 2003, Science.

[30]  E. Moss,et al.  Attachment at school age and academic performance. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[31]  Feng Sheng,et al.  Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses , 2012, NeuroImage.

[32]  D. Nesdale,et al.  Social identity and the development of children's group attitudes. , 2001, Child development.

[33]  Danielle Z. Bolling,et al.  Development of neural systems for processing social exclusion from childhood to adolescence. , 2011, Developmental science.

[34]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  Inhibitory processing during the Go/NoGo task: an ERP analysis of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[35]  Carrie L. Masten,et al.  Do neural responses to rejection depend on attachment style? An fMRI study. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[36]  C. Malatesta,et al.  The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. , 1989, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[37]  Shu-Chen Li,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of selective attention: A lifespan comparison , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.

[38]  K. Hugenberg,et al.  Being “In” With the In-Crowd: The Effects of Social Exclusion and Inclusion Are Enhanced by the Perceived Essentialism of Ingroups and Outgroups , 2010, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[39]  Elizabeth K. Hughes,et al.  The normative development of emotion regulation strategy use in children and adolescents: a 2-year follow-up study. , 2010, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[40]  J. Solomon,et al.  Representational models of relationships: Links between caregiving and attachment , 1996 .

[41]  Michael J Crowley,et al.  Exclusion and micro-rejection: event-related potential response predicts mitigated distress , 2009, Neuroreport.

[42]  M. Lieberman,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging responses relate to differences in real-world social experience. , 2007, Emotion.

[43]  M. Bakermans-Kranenburg,et al.  The first 10,000 Adult Attachment Interviews: distributions of adult attachment representations in clinical and non-clinical groups , 2009, Attachment & human development.

[44]  Sarah-Jayne Blakemore,et al.  Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[45]  N. Eisenberger The Neural Bases of Social Pain: Evidence for Shared Representations With Physical Pain , 2012, Psychosomatic medicine.

[46]  H. E. Cole,et al.  Attachment and emotion regulation during mother-teen problem solving: a control theory analysis. , 1993, Child development.

[47]  S. Coyne,et al.  Adolescents' Prosocial Responses to Ostracism: An Experimental Study , 2011, The Journal of social psychology.

[48]  Marinus H. van IJzendoorn Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the adult attachment interview , 1995 .

[49]  S. Friedlander,et al.  WHEN A CHILD REJECTS A PARENT: TAILORING THE INTERVENTION TO FIT THE PROBLEM , 2010 .

[50]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[51]  M. Leary,et al.  Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  Rick Richardson,et al.  How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence , 2004 .

[53]  Naomi I. Eisenberger,et al.  Identifying the Neural Correlates Underlying Social Pain: Implications for Developmental Processes , 2006, Human Development.

[54]  Guy Dove,et al.  Linking Brainwaves to the Brain: An ERP Primer , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[55]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Does arousal per se account for the influence of appetitive stimuli on attentional scope and the late positive potential? , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[56]  Philip A. Gable,et al.  Late positive potential to appetitive stimuli and local attentional bias. , 2010, Emotion.

[57]  J. Bowlby,et al.  The effects of mother-child separation: a follow-up study. , 1956, The British journal of medical psychology.

[58]  M. Main,et al.  Predicting rejection of her infant from mother's representation of her own experience: implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. , 1984, Child abuse & neglect.

[59]  E. Carlson,et al.  Associations among adult attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant-mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. , 1995, Child development.

[60]  I. Franken,et al.  Event-related potentials indicate motivational relevance of cocaine cues in abstinent cocaine addicts , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[61]  Steven M. Platek,et al.  In-Group and Out-Group Membership Mediates Anterior Cingulate Activation to Social Exclusion , 2009, Frontiers in evolutionary neuroscience.

[62]  H. Harlow,et al.  Social recovery by isolation-reared monkeys. , 1971, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[63]  Thomas R. Insel,et al.  Epigenetic sources of behavioral differences in mice , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[64]  K. Williams,et al.  Bulletin Personality and Social Psychology Need Threat Can Motivate Performance after Ostracism on Behalf Of: Society for Personality and Social Psychology , 2022 .