Reading performance of dyslexics with a special font and a colored background

This research examined if the reading performance of dyslexics and non-dyslexics was positively affected by the adaptation of background color and font. Using a light yellow background could have a positive influence on reading performance in comparison with a white background. Also, the font Dyslexie was compared with the font Arial. According to the magnocellular theory the adaptation of these visual elements can improve the reading performance of dyslexics and non-dyslexics. Measurements of reading performance were speed and accuracy. Twenty-two dyslexics and forty-two non-dyslexics with a lower and higher reading level participated in this study. Every child read four different texts with the following color-font combination: Yellow-Dyslexie, White-Dyslexie, Yellow-Arial and White-Arial. For reading accuracy an interaction was found of the color yellow and the font Dyslexie for dyslexics with a lower reading level. Findings also showed a positive main effect of the font Dyslexie with the reading accuracy for the total group of dyslexics and for dyslexics with a lower reading level. Further research is needed to fully explore the role of background color and level of prior knowledge between dyslexics with a lower and higher reading level and the influence on reading performance. Practical implication could be the implementation of the color yellow and the font Dyslexie for dyslexics with a lower reading level.

[1]  P. Gomez,et al.  The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics , 2012 .

[2]  Pablo Gomez,et al.  Increasing interletter spacing facilitates encoding of words , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[3]  Manuel Perea,et al.  The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Manuel Perea,et al.  Do serifs provide an advantage in the recognition of written words? , 2011 .

[5]  J. Keenan,et al.  How prior knowledge affects word identification and comprehension , 2011, Reading and writing.

[6]  Arnold J Wilkins,et al.  Periodic letter strokes within a word affect fixation disparity during reading. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[7]  Carretera Tijuana-Ensenada,et al.  Comparative analysis between different font types and letter styles using a nonlinear invariant digital correlation , 2010 .

[8]  Renske de Leeuw,et al.  Special font for dyslexia , 2010 .

[9]  Arnold J. Wilkins,et al.  Typography for children may be inappropriately designed , 2009 .

[10]  Axel Buchner,et al.  The advantage of positive text-background polarity is due to high display luminance , 2009, Ergonomics.

[11]  Janet G. van Hell,et al.  Explorations in learning and the brain : on the potential of cognitive neuroschience for education , 2009 .

[12]  J. Wouters,et al.  Modelling relations between sensory processing, speech perception, orthographic and phonological ability, and literacy achievement , 2008, Brain and Language.

[13]  R. A. Floyd,et al.  Color and luminance increment thresholds in poor readers , 2008, Visual Neuroscience.

[14]  K. Amunts,et al.  Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. , 2008, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[15]  Arnold J Wilkins,et al.  Stripes within Words Affect Reading , 2007, Perception.

[16]  Arnold J. Wilkins,et al.  How Many Colours Are Necessary to Increase the Reading Speed of Children with Visual Stress? A Comparison of Two Systems , 2007 .

[17]  Richard E. Mayer,et al.  Learning and Instruction , 2007 .

[18]  T. Gog,et al.  Explorations in learning and the brain: A quick scan of the potential of neuroscience for education , 2007 .

[19]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  "Stroop concordant" coloring of letters for remediation of dyslexia. , 2007, Medical hypotheses.

[20]  Paul van Schaik,et al.  The influence of font type and line length on visual search and information retrieval in web pages , 2006, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[21]  James E. Sheedy,et al.  Text Legibility and the Letter Superiority Effect , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[22]  Aries Arditi,et al.  Serifs and font legibility , 2005, Vision Research.

[23]  S. Fowler,et al.  Yellow Filters Can Improve Magnocellular Function: Motion Sensitivity, Convergence, Accommodation, and Reading , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  Alice F Healy,et al.  A blank look in reading: the effect of blank space on the identification of letters and words during reading. , 2005, Experimental psychology.

[25]  Jean-François Démonet,et al.  Developmental dyslexia , 2004, The Lancet.

[26]  Arthur C. Graesser,et al.  Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[27]  Michelle Waldie,et al.  How big does a coloured overlay have to be? , 2004, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[28]  John Stein,et al.  Visual motion sensitivity and reading , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Barbara S. Chaparro,et al.  Comparing the effects of text size and format on the readibility of computer-displayed Times New Roman and Arial text , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[30]  Jen-Her Wu,et al.  Improving searching and reading performance: the effect of highlighting and text color coding , 2003, Inf. Manag..

[31]  Kristofer Kinsey,et al.  Can the magnocellular pathway read? Evidence from studies of color , 2003, Vision Research.

[32]  B. Shaywitz,et al.  A definition of dyslexia , 2003 .

[33]  D. Spinelli,et al.  Crowding Effects on Word Identification in Developmental Dyslexia , 2002, Cortex.

[34]  D. Willows,et al.  The Incidence and Nature of Letter Orientation Errors in Reading Disability , 2002, Journal of learning disabilities.

[35]  Susana T. L. Chung The effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. , 2002, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[36]  Barbara S. Chaparro,et al.  Examining children's reading performance and preference for different computer-displayed text , 2002, Behav. Inf. Technol..

[37]  A. Wilkins,et al.  Typography in children’s reading schemes may be suboptimal: Evidence from measures of reading rate , 2000 .

[38]  A. Ahumada,et al.  Discriminability measures for predicting readability of text on textured backgrounds. , 2000, Optics express.

[39]  Lauren F. V. Scharff,et al.  Discriminability measures for predicting readability , 1999, Electronic Imaging.

[40]  J. Stein,et al.  VISUAL MAGNOCELLULAR IMPAIRMENT IN ADULT DEVELOPMENTAL DYSLEXICS , 1998 .

[41]  N. A. Badian Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis , 1997 .

[42]  J. Stein,et al.  To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[43]  James Gaston Bloodsworth Legibility of Print. , 1993 .

[44]  F. Sofo,et al.  Reading Disabilities and the Effects of Colored Filters , 1990, Journal of learning disabilities.

[45]  T. Whitlock Dyslexia , 1989, The Lancet.

[46]  L. Leslie,et al.  The Effects of Prior Knowledge and Oral Reading Accuracy on Miscues and Comprehension , 1985 .

[47]  Alexander I. Rudnicky,et al.  Size and case of type as stimuli in reading. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  Gregory R. Lockhead,et al.  Making Letters Distinctive. , 1980 .

[49]  L. Leslie The Use of Graphic and Contextual Information by Average and Below-Average Readers , 1980 .

[50]  James Hartley,et al.  Designing instructional text , 1978 .

[51]  M. Tinker Legibility of print , 1963 .