Ecologically valid defocus blur contributes to avoidance of interocular suppression in half-occluded zones

In this study, the effect of defocus blur on the avoidance of interocular suppression in half-occluded zones, which are the monocular zones formed by occlusion, was examined. Six human adults observed a pair of colored random-dot stereograms, and the cumulative time of disappearance of the half-occluded zones was measured. There were three defocus blur conditions (blurry-background, blurry-occluder, and nonblur) and two fixation conditions (fixation-front and fixation-back). The author found that the cumulative time of disappearance was shorter under the blurrybackground condition of the fixation-front condition and under the blurry-occluder condition of the fixation-back condition than under the other conditions. These results indicate that ecologically valid defocus blur contributes to the avoidance of interocular suppression in the half-occluded zones.

[1]  M. Fahle Binocular rivalry: Suppression depends on orientation and spatial frequency , 1982, Vision Research.

[2]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Leonardo's constraint: two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  J. J. KULIKOWSKI,et al.  Limit of single vision in stereopsis depends on contour sharpness , 1978, Nature.

[4]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Toward a general theory of stereopsis: binocular matching, occluding contours, and fusion. , 1994, Psychological review.

[5]  J P Frisby,et al.  The Computation of Binocular Edges , 1980, Perception.

[6]  W E Grimson,et al.  A computer implementation of a theory of human stereo vision. , 1981, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[7]  C. Erkelens,et al.  Is there an interaction between perceived direction and perceived aspect ratio in stereoscopic vision? , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[9]  Nicholas J. Wade,et al.  Binocular rivalry with moving patterns , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  B. Julesz,et al.  A THEORETICAL ANALYSIS OF ILLUSORY CONTOUR FORMATION IN STEREOPSIS , 1995 .

[11]  B Gillam,et al.  The Role of Monocular Regions in Stereoscopic Displays , 1988, Perception.

[12]  H. Takeichi,et al.  Binocular Displacement of Unpaired Region , 1994, Perception.

[13]  H. Ono,et al.  Ecologically Invalid Monocular Texture Leads to Longer Perceptual Latencies in Random-Dot Stereograms , 1999, Perception.

[14]  Laurie M. Wilcox,et al.  The role of monocularly visible regions in depth and surface perception , 2009, Vision Research.

[15]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  An integrative model of binocular vision: a stereo model utilizing interocularly unpaired points produces both depth and binocular rivalry , 2004, Vision Research.

[16]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Real world occlusion constraints and binocular rivalry , 1990, Vision Research.

[17]  B. Gillam,et al.  Panum's Limiting Case: Double Fusion, Convergence Error, or ‘Da Vinci Stereopsis’ , 1995, Perception.

[18]  W. Levelt The alternation process in binocular rivalry , 1966 .

[19]  Binocular Rivalry with Rotary and Radial Motions , 1986, Perception.