How anesthesia causes amnesia remains unclear. Understanding this is clinically important because amnesia is a fundamental component of a proper general anesthetic. It has long been assumed that drugs cause amnesia by working on the hippocampus.1 This hippocampal-centered view is largely based on the 1957 study by Scoville and Milner, who reported patient H.M. lost the ability to form new memories following bilateral lesions of his hippocampus.2 It follows that if the hippocampus is needed to form new memories and if people on anesthesia can’t form new memories, then anesthetics probably cause amnesia by acting on the hippocampus. Yet, this view of drug-induced amnesia may be too simplistic. The hippocampus does not work in isolation. Indeed, other brain regions such as the basolateral amygdala (BLA) may be important for mediating amnesia.3–6 Presently, the work of Yu Ren and colleagues provides the missing bridge needed to unite the hippocampal-centric view of anesthetic-induced amnesia with an amygdala-centric view, by showing how anesthetic actions in the BLA may influence the hippocampus and modulate memory.7 Understanding this study requires integrating two key facts about memory.
First, memory is a time-dependent process.8 Memories are not formed and stored instantly, rather they take time to consolidate.9 In time-dependent terms, memory is often categorized into one of three types, based on the interval between learning and memory. These time-frames are called: (1) immediate (i.e., seconds), (2) short-term (from seconds to around 60 – 90 min) and (3) long-term (generally > 90 min). New memories exist in short-term memory in a labile state. During this time window, the actions of the BLA can modulate the process of memory consolidation occurring in other brain regions, such as the hippocampus.10 This modulatory aspect of memory allows for a sort-of biological memory filter, whereby the most important experiences tend to be the best remembered. Enhanced amygdala activity, usually through strong emotional arousal, enhances the consolidation process and creates long-lasting memories.9,11 Conversely, suppressing amygdala activity likely hinders consolidation, creating weak - easily forgotten memories. Most drugs seem to exert their amnesic effects on long-term memory through a BLA-dependent mechanism.6,10,12–15
Second, in contrast with short-term memory, long-term memory is thought to involve a change in brain structure at the synaptic level. This learning-related change is called synaptic plasticity. It represents the fact that when two neurons routinely (or strongly) communicate with each other, it becomes easier for them to do so in the future because their synapses are physically changed. To change neuronal structure likely requires de novo synthesis of new cellular proteins. For many cellular genes, and in particular the so-called immediate-early genes, increased protein expression requires synthesis of new messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA) in response to a learning event.16 The induction of mRNA implies a rather complex chain of molecular events has occurred, in order to transform a learning experience from an excitable neuronal membrane receptor signal into a molecular signal that reaches the cell’s nucleus and interacts with its deoxyribonucleic acid (DNA) to “turn on” the genes of memory formation.16 This chain of events has been well-reviewed elsewhere,17 and a summary of the steps involved is shown in figure 1A. Anesthetics could interact with multiple points along this chain, ranging from stopping the initial ligand-receptor interactions, to stopping the cellular processes related to immediate-early gene expression.
Fig. 1
shows a number of molecular mechanisms involved in long-term memory formation. Currently, long-term memory is thought to involve the de novo synthesis or transcription of new mRNA originating from learning-related immediate early genes. The molecular ...
The learning-related gene product measured by Yu Ren and colleagues in the hippocampus of rats trained to avoid a footshock was the mRNA level for activity-regulated cytoskeletal (Arc) associated protein.7 This is only one of a handful of potential memory-related immediate early effector genes,16 but it is an important one that shows many of the characteristics one would anticipate from a learning-related gene. Of these attributes, Arc is implicated in directly altering synaptic function (reviewed in Miyashita et al., 2008),16 and past studies have shown learning induced Arc protein expression in the hippocampus is critical for consolidation of inhibitory avoidance and water maze learning.12,18 Importantly, Yu Ren and colleagues found that when animals were trained on the long-term memory task, even during exposure to an amnesic dose of propofol, the level of Arc mRNA increased in their hippocampus, to the same extent as that expected with normal learning (their fig. 3).16 This strongly suggests that the amnesic effect of propofol does not occur by blocking learning at any stage in the process prior to the transcription of the mRNA for Arc (i.e., this essentially rules out all processes illustrated in fig. 1A). This animal finding integrates well with one recent human study where propofol did not appear to block memory encoding.19 So why did the animals show a behavioral deficit in long-term memory performance during propofol, if their hippocampus still made the proper amount of Arc mRNA? The answer is that propofol caused a reduction in the amount of hippocampal Arc PROTEIN (their fig. 4). This means that the amnesic effect of propofol on long-term memory is likely related to disruption of protein translation (i.e., the mRNA to protein synthesis step). The complex regulatory pathways involved with protein translation are illustrated in figure 1B.20 Within this figure, somewhere is where propofol likely acts to suppress long-term memory.
So, if propofol causes amnesia by blocking Arc protein production in the hippocampus and if we know from prior study that lesions of the BLA remove the amnesic effect of propofol,4 then perhaps it is the effect of propofol on the BLA that causes the suppression of the Arc protein in the hippocampus. Yu Ren and colleagues followed this logic and reasoned that if propofol acts on the BLA as a gamma-aminobutyric acid agonist, then putting a gamma-aminobutyric acid antagonist directly into the BLA should counteract propofol’s amygdala effects and restore both its behavioral memory deficit and its suppression of Arc protein levels in the hippocampus. This is exactly what they found and report in their figure 4. Thus, it appears that propofol’s amnesic effect (at low doses) on long-term (aversive) memory can reasonably be attributed to its gamma-aminobutyric acid like interactions within the BLA.
Is anesthetic-induced suppression of hippocampal Arc protein a common method by which anesthetics act to produce amnesia for long-term aversive memories? Our recent results with sevoflurane and desflurane suggest that it might be. Amnesic doses of both agents were found to suppress hippocampal Arc protein. Our pattern of results follows that found by Yu Ren and colleagues, in that an amnesic dose of sevoflurane did not suppress hippocampal Arc mRNA, but did suppress hippocampal Arc protein.21 Thus, the hypothesis is raised that anesthetic-induced amnesia occurs in part through the suppression of protein products derived from the immediate-early genes that respond to a learning event. It is worth noting that previous studies have shown that post-learning intra-BLA injections of compounds that either enhance or impair long-term memory also function by respectively either increasing or decreasing Arc protein expression in the hippocampus.12 Therefore, the findings of Yu Ren and colleagues can be seen as important further support for the general idea that the amygdala modulates memory consolidation by regulating the expression of plasticity-related gene products in the hippocampus. These findings offer an encouraging theoretical framework and an experimental basis for contemplating the idea that memory erasing drugs might someday form a part of our clinical armamentarium.
[1]
Fu-jun Zhang,et al.
Bilateral Inhibition of γ-Aminobutyric Acid Type A Receptor Function within the Basolateral Amygdala Blocked Propofol-induced Amnesia and Activity-regulated Cytoskeletal Protein Expression Inhibition in the Hippocampus
,
2008,
Anesthesiology.
[2]
Ray Johnson,et al.
Low-dose Propofol–induced Amnesia Is Not due to a Failure of Encoding: Left Inferior Prefrontal Cortex Is Still Active
,
2008,
Anesthesiology.
[3]
John F. Guzowski,et al.
Networks of neurons, networks of genes: An integrated view of memory consolidation
,
2008,
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[4]
Michael T. Alkire,et al.
Neuroimaging analysis of an anesthetic gas that blocks human emotional memory
,
2008,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[5]
J. D. McGaugh,et al.
Memory-influencing intra-basolateral amygdala drug infusions modulate expression of Arc protein in the hippocampus.
,
2005,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[6]
M. Alkire,et al.
Does the Amygdala Mediate Anesthetic-induced Amnesia?: Basolateral Amygdala Lesions Block Sevoflurane-induced Amnesia
,
2005,
Anesthesiology.
[7]
J. Antognini,et al.
Anesthesia, amnesia, and the amygdala: reducing the fear of intraoperative awareness.
,
2005,
Anesthesiology.
[8]
E. Klann,et al.
Biochemical mechanisms for translational regulation in synaptic plasticity
,
2004,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[9]
J. D. McGaugh.
The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences.
,
2004,
Annual review of neuroscience.
[10]
B. Roozendaal.
Systems mediating acute glucocorticoid effects on memory consolidation and retrieval
,
2003,
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.
[11]
L. Cahill,et al.
Lesions of the Basolateral Amygdala Complex Block Propofol-induced Amnesia for Inhibitory Avoidance Learning in Rats
,
2001,
Anesthesiology.
[12]
T. Carew,et al.
Molecular stepping stones in memory consolidation
,
2001,
Nature Neuroscience.
[13]
J. D. McGaugh,et al.
Inhibition of Activity-Dependent Arc Protein Expression in the Rat Hippocampus Impairs the Maintenance of Long-Term Potentiation and the Consolidation of Long-Term Memory
,
2000,
The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14]
J. D. McGaugh.
Memory--a century of consolidation.
,
2000,
Science.
[15]
A. Pohorille,et al.
Hypothesis: Inhaled Anesthetics Produce Immobility and Amnesia by Different Mechanisms at Different Sites
,
1997,
Anesthesia and analgesia.
[16]
J L McGaugh,et al.
Amygdala activity at encoding correlated with long-term, free recall of emotional information.
,
1996,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[17]
J. Mcgaugh,et al.
Basolateral amygdala lesions block diazepam-induced anterograde amnesia in an inhibitory avoidance task.
,
1992,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[18]
J L McGaugh,et al.
Time-Dependent Processes in Memory Storage
,
1966,
Science.
[19]
W. Scoville,et al.
LOSS OF RECENT MEMORY AFTER BILATERAL HIPPOCAMPAL LESIONS
,
1957,
Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[20]
J. D. McGaugh,et al.
Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events.
,
1994,
Nature.