Intact action segmentation in Parkinson's disease: Hypothesis testing using a novel computational approach

Action observation is known to trigger predictions of the ongoing course of action and thus considered a hallmark example for predictive perception. A related task, which explicitly taps into the ability to predict actions based on their internal representations, is action segmentation; the task requires participants to demarcate where one action step is completed and another one begins. It thus benefits from a temporally precise prediction of the current action. Formation and exploitation of these temporal predictions of external events is now closely associated with a network including the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex. Because decline of dopaminergic innervation leads to impaired function of the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex in Parkinson's disease (PD), we hypothesised that PD patients would show increased temporal variability in the action segmentation task, especially under medication withdrawal (hypothesis 1). Another crucial aspect of action segmentation is its reliance on a semantic representation of actions. There is no evidence to suggest that action representations are substantially altered, or cannot be accessed, in non-demented PD patients. We therefore expected action segmentation judgments to follow the same overall patterns in PD patients and healthy controls (hypothesis 2), resulting in comparable segmentation profiles. Both hypotheses were tested with a novel classification approach. We present evidence for both hypotheses in the present study: classifier performance was slightly decreased when it was tested for its ability to predict the identity of movies segmented by PD patients, and a measure of normativity of response behaviour was decreased when patients segmented movies under medication-withdrawal without access to an episodic memory of the sequence. This pattern of results is consistent with hypothesis 1. However, the classifier analysis also revealed that responses given by patients and controls create very similar action-specific patterns, thus delivering evidence in favour hypothesis 2. In terms of methodology, the use of classifiers in the present study allowed us to establish similarity of behaviour across groups (hypothesis 2). The approach opens up a new avenue that standard statistical methods often fail to provide and is discussed in terms of its merits to measure hypothesised similarities across study populations.

[1]  Darren Newtson,et al.  The perceptual organization of ongoing behavior , 1976 .

[2]  Y. Agid,et al.  Dopamine deficiency in the cerebral cortex in Parkinson disease , 1982, Neurology.

[3]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold your horses: A dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[4]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective , 2009, Cognition.

[5]  Richard Camicioli,et al.  Parkinson's disease is associated with hippocampal atrophy , 2003, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[6]  Kevin P. Murphy,et al.  Machine learning - a probabilistic perspective , 2012, Adaptive computation and machine learning series.

[7]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event Segmentation , 2007, Current directions in psychological science.

[8]  Anne Springer,et al.  Predicting and memorizing observed action: Differential premotor cortex involvement , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[9]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Human brain activity time-locked to perceptual event boundaries , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  Brian Colder,et al.  Emulation as an Integrating Principle for Cognition , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[11]  Myrna F Schwartz,et al.  The cognitive neuropsychology of everyday action and planning , 2006, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[12]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Understanding decision-making deficits in neurological conditions: insights from models of natural action selection , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Medial Temporal Lobe Volume Predicts Elders’ Everyday Memory , 2013, Psychological science.

[14]  M. Frank,et al.  Anatomy of a decision: striato-orbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. , 2006, Psychological review.

[15]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Left Inferior Parietal Representations for Skilled Hand-Object Interactions: Evidence from Stroke and Corticobasal Degeneration , 2007, Cortex.

[16]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[17]  R. Miall,et al.  Interference Effects from Observed Movement in Parkinson's Disease , 2010, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  R. Schubotz,et al.  Caudate Nucleus Signals for Breaches of Expectation in a Movement Observation Paradigm , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[19]  P. Alm Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. , 2004, Journal of communication disorders.

[20]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Deficit in Evaluating Pre-Determinated Sequences of Script Events in Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 1998, Cortex.

[21]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[22]  Franziska M. Korb,et al.  The fraction of an action is more than a movement: Neural signatures of event segmentation in fMRI , 2012, NeuroImage.

[23]  Douglas P. Munoz,et al.  Executive impairment in Parkinson's disease: Response automaticity and task switching , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  E. Procyk,et al.  Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  N. Yeung,et al.  The role of prediction and outcomes in adaptive cognitive control , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[27]  M. Arbib,et al.  Movement Inhibition and Next Sensory State Prediction in the Basal Ganglia , 2002 .

[28]  L. Henderson,et al.  Predictive responses in Parkinson's disease: manual keypresses and saccadic eye movements to regular stimulus events. , 1989, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[29]  Anne B. Kühn,et al.  Joint principles of motor and cognitive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[30]  A. Lees,et al.  The relationship between dementia and direct involvement of the hippocampus and amygdala in Parkinson's disease , 1997, Neurology.

[31]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  J. Grafman The Structured Event Complex and the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2009 .

[33]  M. Emre Dementia associated with Parkinson's disease , 2003, The Lancet Neurology.

[34]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event perception: A theory and its application to clinical neuroscience. , 2010 .

[35]  Christopher A. Kurby,et al.  Aging and the segmentation of narrative film , 2014, Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition.

[36]  Christine E. Watson,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of actions , 2011, Neurology.

[37]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A Computational Model of How the Basal Ganglia Produce Sequences , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  P. A. Lewis,et al.  Brain activity correlates differentially with increasing temporal complexity of rhythms during initialisation, synchronisation, and continuation phases of paced finger tapping , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Jacob Cohen The earth is round (p < .05) , 1994 .

[40]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Event understanding and memory in healthy aging and dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 2006, Psychology and aging.

[41]  Eduardo A. B. da Silva,et al.  Digital Signal Processing System Analysis and Design: Introduction , 2010 .

[42]  E. Poliakoff Representation of action in Parkinson's disease: imagining, observing, and naming actions. , 2013, Journal of neuropsychology.

[43]  Ricarda I Schubotz,et al.  Neural changes when actions change: Adaptation of strong and weak expectations , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[44]  V Kaasinen,et al.  Hippocampal and prefrontal atrophy in patients with early non-demented Parkinson’s disease is related to cognitive impairment , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[45]  P. Riederer,et al.  Time course of nigrostriatal degeneration in parkinson's disease , 1976, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[46]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[47]  Robert P. Spunt,et al.  Dissociable Neural Systems Support Retrieval of How and Why Action Knowledge , 2010, Psychological science.

[48]  T. Robbins,et al.  l-Dopa medication remediates cognitive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  R. Cools Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function-implications for l-DOPA treatment in Parkinson's disease , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Enhanced or impaired cognitive function in Parkinson's disease as a function of dopaminergic medication and task demands. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  G. Csibra Action mirroring and action understanding: an alternative account , 1993 .

[52]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Segmenting dynamic human action via statistical structure , 2008, Cognition.

[53]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Segmentation in the perception and memory of events , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  M. Gluck,et al.  Basal ganglia and dopamine contributions to probabilistic category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[55]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The mirror-neuron system: a Bayesian perspective. , 2007, Neuroreport.

[56]  K. Jellinger,et al.  Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington. Clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. , 1973, Journal of the neurological sciences.

[57]  Roy W Jones,et al.  The degraded concept representation system in semantic dementia: damage to pan-modal hub, then visual spoke. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  Pasquale Calabrese,et al.  Screening for cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease with the Parkinson neuropsychometric dementia assessment (PANDA) instrument , 2008 .

[59]  C. Lebiere,et al.  Conditional routing of information to the cortex: a model of the basal ganglia's role in cognitive coordination. , 2010, Psychological review.

[60]  Kenneth W. Flowers,et al.  Lack of prediction in the motor behaviour of Parkinsonism. , 1978, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[61]  A. Nobre,et al.  Temporal Expectation Improves the Quality of Sensory Information , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  T. Jay Dopamine: a potential substrate for synaptic plasticity and memory mechanisms , 2003, Progress in Neurobiology.

[63]  B. Balleine,et al.  The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  A. Dagher,et al.  The role of the striatum and hippocampus in planning: a PET activation study in Parkinson's disease. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  D. Plaut,et al.  Doing without schema hierarchies: a recurrent connectionist approach to normal and impaired routine sequential action. , 2004, Psychological review.