Irrelevant Singletons in Visual Search Do Not Capture Attention but Can Produce Nonspatial Filtering Costs
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Remington,et al. Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Codes and their vicissitudes , 2001 .
[3] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] R. Remington,et al. Top-down modulation of preattentive processing: Testing the recovery account of contingent capture , 2006 .
[6] S. Luck,et al. Attention-Related Modulation of Sensory-Evoked Brain Activity in a Visual Search Task , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[7] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[8] Erich Schröger,et al. Texture segmentation and visual search for pop-out targets. An ERP study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[9] J. Duncan,et al. Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.
[10] Agnieszka Wykowska,et al. Detecting pop-out targets in contexts of varying homogeneity: Investigating homogeneity coding with event-related brain potentials (ERPs) , 2007, Brain Research.
[11] A. Allport,et al. Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .
[12] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[13] Jeff Miller,et al. Jackknife-based method for measuring LRP onset latency differences. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[14] J. Theeuwes,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[15] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[16] H. Müller,et al. Searching for unknown feature targets on more than one dimension: Investigating a “dimension-weighting” account , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[17] Eric Ruthruff,et al. Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: converging evidence from event-related potentials. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Space and selective attention , 1994 .
[19] K. Cave,et al. Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[20] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[21] H. J. Muller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] Geoffrey F Woodman,et al. Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] K. Cave. The FeatureGate model of visual selection , 1999, Psychological research.
[24] S. Luck,et al. The role of attention in feature detection and conjunction discrimination: an electrophysiological analysis. , 1995, The International journal of neuroscience.
[25] George R. Mangun,et al. Reflexive Attention Modulates Processing of Visual Stimuli in Human Extrastriate Cortex , 1998, Psychological science.
[26] M. Eimer. The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[27] Martin Eimer,et al. Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] Agnieszka Wykowska,et al. On the Temporal Relation of Top–Down and Bottom–Up Mechanisms during Guidance of Attention , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[29] H. Heuer,et al. Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .
[30] S. Yantis,et al. Mechanisms of attentional selection: Temporally modulated priority tags , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] J. Theeuwes,et al. On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .
[32] G. Aschersleben,et al. The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[33] Mathias Weymar,et al. Specific fear modulates attentional selectivity during visual search: electrophysiological insights from the N2pc. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[34] J. Wolfe,et al. Changing your mind: on the contributions of top-down and bottom-up guidance in visual search for feature singletons. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] R. Desimone,et al. Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .
[37] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] Thomas Töllner,et al. Top-down dimensional weight set determines the capture of visual attention: evidence from the PCN component. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[39] H. Egeth,et al. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.