Geometrical haptic illusions revisited: Haptic illusions compared with visual illusions

Révész (1934) reported that haptic illusions were observed in almost all of the geometrical optical illusion figures. The present study reexamined seven geometrical illusions in both haptic and visual modes. In the Müller-Lyer, Ponzo, and vertical-horizontal figures, haptic illusions equivalent to the visual illusions were observed. In the Oppel-Kundt figure, a haptic illusion similar to the visual one was obtained. In the haptic Delboeuf stimuli, the size illusion of the outer circle occurred, whereas that of the inner circle did not. No haptic illusion was obtained in the Poggendorff figure. In the Zöllner figure, a haptic illusion directionally opposite to the visual one was obtained. These results show that haptic illusions do not occur in all of the geometrical illusion figures. They also suggest that haptic illusions are not necessarily mediated by visualization and that haptic processing of the figures often occurs in a manner different from vision.

[1]  L Riggio,et al.  Some observations on the Poggendorff and Müller-Lyer tactual illusions , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  J. Frisby,et al.  Is the Haptic Müller–Lyer a Visual Phenomenon? , 1971, Nature.

[3]  Haptic Illusions and Inappropriate Constancy Scaling , 1967, Nature.

[4]  R. Gregory,et al.  Distortion of Visual Space as Inappropriate Constancy Scaling , 1963, Nature.

[5]  R. H. Day,et al.  Radial and tangential movement directions as determinants of the haptic illusion in an L figure. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  S. Coren,et al.  An empirical taxonomy of visual illusions , 1976 .

[7]  Ray Over A Comparison of Haptic and Visual Judgments of Some Illusions , 1966 .

[8]  T S Wong,et al.  Dynamic properties of radial and tangential movements as determinants of the haptic horizontal--vertical illusion with an L figure. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  System der optischen und haptischen Raumtäuschungen. , 1934 .

[10]  R. Klatzky,et al.  There's more to touch than meets the eye: The salience of object attributes for haptics with and without vision. , 1987 .

[11]  Charles H. Bean The blind have "optical illusions." , 1938 .

[12]  J. Ogasawara DISPALCEMENT-EFFECT OF CONCENTRIC CIRCLES , 1952 .

[13]  S Appelle,et al.  Effect of Modality-Specific Experience on Visual and Haptic Judgment of Orientation , 1985, Perception.

[14]  C. Parrish The Cutaneous Estimation of Open and Filled Space , 1895 .

[15]  Rita G. Rudel,et al.  Decrement of Visual and Haptic Müller-lyer Illusion on Repeated Trials: A Study of Crossmodal Transfer , 1963 .

[16]  Variations in the Illusion of Filled and Unfilled Tactual Space , 1931 .

[17]  D. Alais,et al.  Lack of evidence for a tactual Poggendorff illusion , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Y HATWELL [Study of several geometrical tactile illusions in the blind]. , 1960, L'annee psychologique.

[19]  Tactual Poggendorff Illusion in Blind and Blindfolded Subjects , 1970, Perceptual and motor skills.

[20]  M F Cheng,et al.  Tactile-kinesthetic perception of length. , 1968, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  Y. Hatwell,et al.  Etude de quelques illusions géométriques tactiles chez les aveugles , 1960 .

[22]  J. Kershner,et al.  Lateral Preference and Ability to Conserve Multiple Spatial Relations by Mentally Retarded Children , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.

[23]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Size interactions in the perception of orientation , 1983 .

[24]  L. S. Tsai Mueller-Lyer Illusion by the Blind , 1967, Perceptual and motor skills.

[25]  P. Wenderoth,et al.  Relationships between the kinetic, alternating-line, and Poggendorff illusions: The effects of interstimulus interval, inducing parallels, and fixation , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  James Patterson,et al.  Haptic Perception of the Mueller-Lyer Illusion by the Blind , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.