Language Exposure Relates to Structural Neural Connectivity in Childhood

Neuroscience research has elucidated broad relationships between socioeconomic status (SES) and young children's brain structure, but there is little mechanistic knowledge about specific environmental factors that are associated with specific variation in brain structure. One environmental factor, early language exposure, predicts children's linguistic and cognitive skills and later academic achievement, but how language exposure relates to neuroanatomy is unknown. By measuring the real-world language exposure of young children (ages 4–6 years, 27 male/13 female), we confirmed the preregistered hypothesis that greater adult-child conversational experience, independent of SES and the sheer amount of adult speech, is related to stronger, more coherent white matter connectivity in the left arcuate and superior longitudinal fasciculi on average, and specifically near their anterior termination at Broca's area in left inferior frontal cortex. Fractional anisotropy of significant tract subregions mediated the relationship between conversational turns and children's language skills and indicated a neuroanatomical mechanism underlying the SES “language gap.” Post hoc whole-brain analyses revealed that language exposure was not related to any other white matter tracts, indicating the specificity of this relationship. Results suggest that the development of dorsal language tracts is environmentally influenced, specifically by early, dialogic interaction. Furthermore, these findings raise the possibility that early intervention programs aiming to ameliorate disadvantages in development due to family SES may focus on increasing children's conversational exposure to capitalize on the early neural plasticity underlying cognitive development. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Over the last decade, cognitive neuroscience has highlighted the detrimental impact of disadvantaged backgrounds on young children's brain structure. However, to intervene effectively, we must know which proximal aspects of the environmental aspects are most strongly related to neural development. The present study finds that young children's real-world language exposure, and specifically the amount of adult-child conversation, correlates with the strength of connectivity in the left hemisphere white matter pathway connecting two canonical language regions, independent of socioeconomic status and the sheer volume of adult speech. These findings suggest that early intervention programs aiming to close the achievement gap may focus on increasing children's conversational exposure to capitalize on the early neural plasticity underlying cognitive development.

[1]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Spurious group differences due to head motion in a diffusion MRI study , 2013, NeuroImage.

[2]  Nicolas Langer,et al.  White Matter Alterations in Infants at Risk for Developmental Dyslexia , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al.  Nipype: A Flexible, Lightweight and Extensible Neuroimaging Data Processing Framework in Python , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[4]  Seth Pettie,et al.  Mind the gap , 2006, Nature Reviews Drug Discovery.

[5]  Anne Fernald,et al.  Talking to Children Matters , 2013, Psychological science.

[6]  A. Bryk,et al.  Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. , 1991 .

[7]  J. Wouters,et al.  A tractography study in dyslexia: neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  A. Hayes Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach , 2013 .

[9]  Sara B. Johnson,et al.  State of the Art Review: Poverty and the Developing Brain , 2016, Pediatrics.

[10]  W. Thomas Boyce,et al.  The Impact of Social Disparity on Prefrontal Function in Childhood , 2012, PloS one.

[11]  John H L Hansen,et al.  Mapping the Early Language Environment Using All-Day Recordings and Automated Analysis. , 2017, American journal of speech-language pathology.

[12]  F. Ramus,et al.  Vocabulary growth rate from preschool to school-age years is reflected in the connectivity of the arcuate fasciculus in 14-year-old children. , 2018, Developmental science.

[13]  Derek K. Jones,et al.  Symmetries in human brain language pathways correlate with verbal recall , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Family Income, Parental Education and Brain Structure in Children and Adolescents , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  M. Rowe,et al.  Predictors of Variation in Maternal Talk to Children: A Longitudinal Study of Low-Income Families , 2005 .

[16]  Michelle L. McGillion,et al.  A randomised controlled trial to test the effect of promoting caregiver contingent talk on language development in infants from diverse socioeconomic status backgrounds , 2017, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[17]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success , 2015, Psychological science.

[18]  Jack L. Vevea,et al.  The varieties of speech to young children. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[19]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[20]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference , 2009, NeuroImage.

[21]  Joanna A. Christodoulou,et al.  Socioeconomic Status and Reading Disability: Neuroanatomy and Plasticity in Response to Intervention , 2017, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Jens Brauer,et al.  Brain Functional and Structural Predictors of Language Performance. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  John H. Gilmore,et al.  Correction: Family Poverty Affects the Rate of Human Infant Brain Growth , 2015, PloS one.

[24]  M. Farah,et al.  Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. , 2005, Developmental science.

[25]  Patricia K Kuhl,et al.  Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. , 2016, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[26]  Jeffrey R. Harring,et al.  A Brief Parent-Focused Intervention to Improve Preschoolers’ Conversational Skills and School Readiness , 2018, Developmental psychology.

[27]  Erika Hoff,et al.  Socioeconomic Status and Parenting , 2019, Handbook of Parenting.

[28]  Jonathan M. Campbell,et al.  Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test , 2010 .

[29]  N. Gaab,et al.  Development of Tract‐Specific White Matter Pathways During Early Reading Development in At‐Risk Children and Typical Controls , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  Daniel J. Miller,et al.  Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  Bruce Fischl,et al.  Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration , 2009, NeuroImage.

[32]  Brian A. Wandell,et al.  Anatomical Properties of the Arcuate Fasciculus Predict Phonological and Reading Skills in Children , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  J. Wouters,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews a Qualitative and Quantitative Review of Diffusion Tensor Imaging Studies in Reading and Dyslexia , 2022 .

[34]  N. Fox,et al.  Effect of foster care on language learning at eight years: Findings from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project* , 2012, Journal of Child Language.

[35]  Psychologie Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence , 2011, Definitions.

[36]  Rebecca J. Panagos Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children , 1998 .

[37]  Seth Warschausky,et al.  Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence , 2018 .

[38]  D. Barch,et al.  The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. , 2013, JAMA pediatrics.

[39]  Antoni Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Word learning is mediated by the left arcuate fasciculus , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  J. Gilkerson,et al.  Teaching by Listening: The Importance of Adult-Child Conversations to Language Development , 2009, Pediatrics.

[41]  Daniel Rueckert,et al.  Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data , 2006, NeuroImage.

[42]  D. Perani,et al.  Neural language networks at birth , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  A. Friederici The cortical language circuit: from auditory perception to sentence comprehension , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Jeffrey T Duda,et al.  Associations between children's socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. , 2013, Developmental science.

[45]  Franck Ramus,et al.  The Influence of Socioeconomic Status on Children’s Brain Structure , 2012, PloS one.

[46]  Joann P. Benigno,et al.  Effect of foster care on young children's language learning. , 2011, Child development.

[47]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children , 2008, NeuroImage.

[48]  D. Perani,et al.  Dorsal and ventral pathways in language development , 2013, Brain and Language.

[49]  C. Lebel,et al.  A review of diffusion MRI of typical white matter development from early childhood to young adulthood , 2019, NMR in biomedicine.

[50]  David E. Osher,et al.  Tracking the Roots of Reading Ability: White Matter Volume and Integrity Correlate with Phonological Awareness in Prereading and Early-Reading Kindergarten Children , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus revealed with comparative DTI , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[52]  Julia A. Leonard,et al.  Neuroanatomical Correlates of the Income-Achievement Gap , 2015, Psychological science.

[53]  M. Rowe A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. , 2012, Child development.

[54]  Kimberly G. Noble,et al.  Socioeconomic status, white matter, and executive function in children , 2016, Brain and behavior.

[55]  Margaret A. Sheridan,et al.  Neglect as a Violation of Species-Expectant Experience: Neurodevelopmental Consequences , 2017, Biological Psychiatry.

[56]  J. Navarro-González,et al.  Inflammatory pathways. , 2011, Contributions to nephrology.

[57]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Automated Probabilistic Reconstruction of White-Matter Pathways in Health and Disease Using an Atlas of the Underlying Anatomy , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[58]  Ninon Burgos,et al.  New advances in the Clinica software platform for clinical neuroimaging studies , 2019 .

[59]  Rachel R. Romeo,et al.  Beyond the 30-Million-Word Gap: Children’s Conversational Exposure Is Associated With Language-Related Brain Function , 2018, Psychological science.

[60]  S. Suter Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children , 2005, European Journal of Pediatrics.

[61]  Daniel S. Margulies,et al.  Nipype: A Flexible, Lightweight and Extensible Neuroimaging Data Processing Framework in Python , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[62]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. , 2007, Developmental science.

[63]  Peter J Gianaros,et al.  Inflammatory pathways link socioeconomic inequalities to white matter architecture. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  M. Catani,et al.  The arcuate fasciculus and the disconnection theme in language and aphasia: History and current state , 2008, Cortex.

[65]  E. Cartmill Mind the Gap , 2016 .

[66]  M. Farah The Neuroscience of Socioeconomic Status: Correlates, Causes, and Consequences , 2017, Neuron.

[67]  C. Tamis-LeMonda,et al.  Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: associations with children's vocabulary and literacy skills at prekindergarten. , 2011, Child Development.

[68]  Marc W. Hernandez,et al.  A parent-directed language intervention for children of low socioeconomic status: a randomized controlled pilot study* , 2015, Journal of Child Language.

[69]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. , 2014, Child development.

[70]  Eric Kan,et al.  Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. , 2012, Developmental science.

[71]  Martin Styner,et al.  DTIPrep: quality control of diffusion-weighted images , 2014, Front. Neuroinform..

[72]  P. Kuhl Is speech learning 'gated' by the social brain? , 2007, Developmental science.