Discounting Financial Literacy: Time Preferences and Participation in Financial Education Programs

Many policy makers and economists argue that financial literacy is key to financial well-being. But why do many individuals remain financially illiterate despite the apparent importance of being financially informed? This paper presents results of a field study linking individual decisions to acquire personal financial information to a critical, and normally unobservable, characteristic: time preferences. We offered a short, free credit counseling and information program to more than 870 individuals. About 55 percent chose to participate. Independently, we elicited time preferences using incentivized choice experiments both for individuals who selected into the program and those who did not. Our results show that the two groups differ sharply in their measured discount factors. Individuals who choose to acquire personal financial information through the credit counseling program discount the future less than individuals who choose not to participate. Our results suggest that individual time preference may explain who will and who will not choose to become financially literate. This has implications for the validity of studies evaluating voluntary financial education programs and policy efforts focused on expanding financial education.

[1]  Jeremy Tobacman,et al.  Payday Loans, Uncertainty and Discounting: Explaining Patterns of Borrowing, Repayment, and Default , 2008 .

[2]  N. Sicherman,et al.  Why Do Dancers Smoke? Smoking, Time Preference, and Wage Dynamics , 1999 .

[3]  D. Ariely,et al.  Doing Good or Doing Well? Image Motivation and Monetary Incentives in Behaving Prosocially , 2007, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[4]  J. Heckman,et al.  Three observations on wages and measured cognitive ability , 2001 .

[5]  John Ifcher,et al.  Happiness and Time Preference: The Effect of Positive Affect in a Random-Assignment Experiment , 2011 .

[6]  S. Meier,et al.  Impatience and Credit Behavior: Using Choice Experiments to Explain Borrowing and Defaulting , 2007 .

[7]  Bart H. H. Golsteyn,et al.  Imagination, Time Discounting and Human Capital Investments , 2006 .

[8]  David Laibson,et al.  Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior , 2008, Journal of risk and uncertainty.

[9]  M. Rabin Risk Aversion and Expected Utility Theory: A Calibration Theorem , 2000 .

[10]  E. Duflo,et al.  The Role of Information and Social Interactions in Retirement Plan Decisions: Evidence from a Randomized Experiment , 2002 .

[11]  Morten I. Lau,et al.  Estimating Individual Discount Rates in Denmark: A Field Experiment , 2002 .

[12]  U. Ben-Zion,et al.  Discount rates inferred from decisions: an experimental study , 1989 .

[13]  Ragan Petrie,et al.  Learning from mistakes: What do inconsistent choices over risk tell us? , 2009 .

[14]  Annamaria Lusardi,et al.  Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparedness: Evidence and Implications for Financial Education , 2007 .

[15]  Petra E. Todd,et al.  Matching As An Econometric Evaluation Estimator: Evidence from Evaluating a Job Training Programme , 1997 .

[16]  Paul S. Calem,et al.  An overview of consumer data and credit reporting , 2003 .

[17]  G. Harrison,et al.  Field Experiments in Economics , 2005 .

[18]  A. Falk,et al.  Individual Risk Attitudes: Measurement, Determinants and Behavioral Consequences , 2009 .

[19]  Casey B. Mulligan,et al.  The Endogenous Determination of Time Preference , 1997 .

[20]  M. Coller,et al.  Eliciting Individual Discount Rates , 1999 .

[21]  Uwe Sunde,et al.  Individual Risk Attitudes: New Evidence from a Large, Representative, Experimentally-Validated Survey , 2005, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[22]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[23]  Paola Sapienza,et al.  Time Discounting for Primary and Monetary Rewards , 2010 .

[24]  Charles Sprenger,et al.  Estimating Time Preferences from Convex Budgets , 2010 .

[25]  Shawn Cole,et al.  If You Are So Smart, Why Arent You Rich? The E¤ects of Education, Financial Literacy and Cognitive Ability on Financial Market Participation , 1997 .

[26]  Catherine C. Eckel,et al.  SAVING DECISIONS OF THE WORKING POOR: SHORT- AND LONG-TERM HORIZONS , 2005 .

[27]  Aldo Rustichini,et al.  Cognitive skills affect economic preferences, strategic behavior, and job attachment , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  Robert Slonim,et al.  Patience among children , 2007 .

[29]  O. Mitchell,et al.  Baby Boomer Retirement Security: The Roles of Planning, Financial Literacy, and Housing Wealth , 2006 .

[30]  S. Meier,et al.  The Cost of Not Knowing the Score: Self-Estimated Credit Scores and Financial Outcomes , 2011 .

[31]  Stephan Meier,et al.  Impatience and Credit Behavior: Evidence from a Field Experiment , 2007 .

[32]  P. Ferraro,et al.  The Today and Tomorrow of Kids , 2008 .

[33]  C. González-Vega,et al.  Evidence on the effect of credit counseling on mortgage loan default by low-income households , 2006 .

[34]  Glenn W. Harrison,et al.  Eliciting risk and time preferences using field experiments: Some methodological issues , 2006 .

[35]  Andrew Caplin,et al.  Wealth Accumulation and the Propensity to Plan , 2002 .

[36]  Stephan Meier,et al.  Present-Biased Preferences and Credit Card Borrowing , 2009, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[37]  S. Meier,et al.  Financial Literacy and Subprime Mortgage Delinquency: Evidence from a Survey Matched to Administrative Data , 2010 .

[38]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[39]  Morten I. Lau,et al.  Eliciting Risk and Time Preferences , 2008 .

[40]  Raphael W. Bostic,et al.  Credit risk, credit scoring, and the performance of home mortgages , 1996 .

[41]  Education and Saving: The Long-Term Effects of High School Financial Curriculum Mandates , 1997 .

[42]  David A. Jaeger,et al.  Direct Evidence on Risk Attitudes and Migration , 2007, The Review of Economics and Statistics.

[43]  Stephan Meier,et al.  Time Discounting Predicts Creditworthiness , 2012, Psychological science.

[44]  B. Bernanke Financial literacy: testimony before the Committee on Banking, Housing, and Urban Affairs, U.S. Senate, May 23, 2006 , 2006 .

[45]  O. Mitchell,et al.  Financial Literacy and Retirement Preparedness: Evidence and Implications for Financial Education Programs , 2007 .

[46]  Sendhil Mullainathan,et al.  A Behavioral-Economics View of Poverty , 2004 .

[47]  Alan Greenspan,et al.  Testimony before the Committee on Banking, Housing, and Urban Affairs, United States Senate , 1987 .

[48]  Bart H. H. Golsteyn,et al.  Imagination, Time Discounting and Human Capital Investment Decisions , 2005 .

[49]  C. Chabris,et al.  Measuring Intertemporal Preferences Using Response Times , 2008 .

[50]  Dean S. Karlan,et al.  Tying Odysseus to the Mast: Evidence from a Commitment Savings Product in the Philippines , 2005 .

[51]  Werner Güth,et al.  On the Interaction of Risk and Time Preferences: An Experimental Study , 2001 .

[52]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Risk and time preferences: linking experimental and household survey data from Vietnam , 2010 .

[53]  S. Braunstein,et al.  Financial literacy: an overview of practice, research, and policy , 2002 .

[54]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review , 2002 .

[55]  Mark B. Stewart,et al.  On Least Squares Estimation when the Dependent Variable is Grouped , 1982 .

[56]  A. Lusardi Financial Literacy and Financial Education: Review and Policy Implications , 2006 .

[57]  M. Staten,et al.  GREGORY ELLIEHAUSEN, E. CHRISTOPHER LUNDQUIST, AND MICHAEL E. STATEN The Impact of Credit Counseling on Subsequent Borrower Behavior , 2007 .

[58]  Jesse M. Shapiro,et al.  WHO IS 'BEHAVIORAL'? COGNITIVE ABILITY AND ANOMALOUS PREFERENCES. , 2013, Journal of the European Economic Association.