Interactive Psychometrics for Autism with the Human Dynamic Clamp: Interpersonal Synchrony from Sensory-motor to Socio-cognitive Domains

Background: The Human Dynamic Clamp (HDC) is a human-machine interface for studying realistic social interaction under controlled and reproducible conditions. Here, we propose to probe the validity of the HDC as psychometric instrument for quantifying social abilities in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASDs) and neurotypical development. Methods: To study behavioral synchrony, we derived from interaction with the HDC avatar, five standardized scores following a gradient from sensory-motor and motor to higher socio-cognitive skills, in a sample of 155 individuals (113 with ASDs, 42 typically developing participants; aged 5 to 25 years; IQ>70). Results: We conducted regression analyses with normative modeling on global scores according to four sub-conditions (avatar behavior "cooperative/competitive", human task "in-phase/anti-phase", diagnosis and age at inclusion). Children with ASDs tend to have significant lower scores than controls for motor skills. Independently of the phenotype, socio-cognitive skills increase with developmental age, while being affected by the ongoing task and the behavior of the avatar. Discussion: The weaker performance in participants with ASDs for motor skills suggests convergent validity for this score of the HDC during social interaction. Results provide additional evidence of a relationship between sensory-motor and socio-cognitive skills. As we found a significant main effect of age at inclusion, HDC may be used as a marker of aging of socio-cognitive skills during real-time social interaction. Conclusion: Through its standardized and objective evaluation, the HDC not only represents a valid paradigm for the study of interpersonal synchrony but also a clinically relevant psychometric instrument for the evaluation and stratification of socio-motor dysfunctions.

[1]  Giacomo Vivanti,et al.  Predictors of Outcomes in Autism Early Intervention: Why Don’t We Know More? , 2014, Front. Pediatr..

[2]  V. Kuhlmeier,et al.  Intention Attribution and the Development of Moral Evaluation , 2019, Front. Psychol..

[3]  S. Ozonoff,et al.  Onset patterns in autism: Variation across informants, methods, and timing , 2018, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[4]  A. Couteur,et al.  Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders , 1994, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[5]  S. Webb,et al.  Infant social attention: an endophenotype of ASD‐related traits? , 2017, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[6]  S. Bölte,et al.  Dissecting the Heterogeneous Cortical Anatomy of Autism Spectrum Disorder Using Normative Models , 2018, bioRxiv.

[7]  V. Eapen,et al.  Clinical outcomes of an early intervention program for preschool children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in a community group setting , 2013, BMC Pediatrics.

[8]  Giacomo Vivanti,et al.  Mechanisms of Imitation Impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2014, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[9]  E. Fombonne The prevalence of autism. , 2003, JAMA.

[10]  Malika Charrad,et al.  NbClust: An R Package for Determining the Relevant Number of Clusters in a Data Set , 2014 .

[11]  R. Newport,et al.  Visuo-tactile integration in autism: atypical temporal binding may underlie greater reliance on proprioceptive information , 2015, Molecular Autism.

[12]  M. Westerfield,et al.  Motor skill in autism spectrum disorders: a subcortical view. , 2013, International review of neurobiology.

[13]  Matthias Bethge,et al.  Natter: A Python Natural Image Statistics Toolbox , 2014 .

[14]  Shihui Han,et al.  Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. , 2016, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[15]  C. Hass,et al.  Motor Coordination in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Synthesis and Meta-Analysis , 2010, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[16]  J. Xavier,et al.  A developmental and clinical perspective of rhythmic interpersonal coordination: From mimicry toward the interconnection of minds , 2016, Journal of Physiology-Paris.

[17]  D. DeSteno,et al.  The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability , 2010 .

[18]  Elizabeth A Stuart,et al.  Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism spectrum disorders: the first 3 years. , 2013, Child development.

[19]  T. Bourgeron,et al.  Gender differences in autism spectrum disorders: Divergence among specific core symptoms , 2017, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[20]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Interpersonal Synchrony: A Survey of Evaluation Methods across Disciplines , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing.

[21]  R. Landa,et al.  Current Perspectives on Motor Functioning in Infants, Children, and Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2011, Physical Therapy.

[22]  R. Gardner,et al.  The Purdue pegboard: Normative data on 1334 school children , 1979 .

[23]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk , 2014, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[24]  Joanna Korman,et al.  Prior Knowledge, Episodic Control and Theory of Mind in Autism: Toward an Integrative Account of Social Cognition , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[25]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  Virtual Partner Interaction (VPI): Exploring Novel Behaviors via Coordination Dynamics , 2009, PloS one.

[26]  A. G. Navarro,et al.  Autismo y cociente intelectual: ¿estabilidad? , 2018 .

[27]  K. Jarrod Millman,et al.  Python for Scientists and Engineers , 2011, Comput. Sci. Eng..

[28]  Jess E. Reynolds,et al.  Prevalence of Motor Difficulties in Autism Spectrum Disorder: Analysis of a Population‐Based Cohort , 2020, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[29]  Elodie Ey,et al.  The genetic landscapes of autism spectrum disorders. , 2013, Annual review of genomics and human genetics.

[30]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  Talma Hendler,et al.  Specifying the Neurobiological Basis of Human Attachment: Brain, Hormones, and Behavior in Synchronous and Intrusive Mothers , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[32]  L. Kanner,et al.  Early infantile autism. , 1944, Pediatric clinics of North America.

[33]  T. Bourgeron,et al.  Alpha Waves as a Neuromarker of Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Challenge of Reproducibility and Heterogeneity , 2018, Front. Neurosci..

[34]  P. Mundy,et al.  Joint-Attention and the Social Phenotype of School-Aged Children with ASD , 2017, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[35]  E. Stuart,et al.  Intervention targeting development of socially synchronous engagement in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. , 2011, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[36]  Aubyn C. Stahmer,et al.  Inclusion for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders , 2011, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[37]  Mengsen Zhang,et al.  Enhanced emotional responses during social coordination with a virtual partner. , 2016, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  S. Ozonoff,et al.  Changing conceptualizations of regression: What prospective studies reveal about the onset of autism spectrum disorder , 2019, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[39]  S. Lind,et al.  Metacognitive monitoring and control processes in children with autism spectrum disorder: Diminished judgement of confidence accuracy , 2016, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  L. Schieve,et al.  Prevalence of Co-occurring Medical and Behavioral Conditions/Symptoms Among 4- and 8-Year-Old Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Selected Areas of the United States in 2010 , 2018, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[41]  Jorge V. José,et al.  Autism: the micro-movement perspective , 2013, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[42]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Autism and social disconnection in interpersonal rocking , 2013, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[43]  E. Torres,et al.  Stochastic Signatures of Involuntary Head Micro-movements Can Be Used to Classify Females of ABIDE into Different Subtypes of Neurodevelopmental Disorders , 2017, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[44]  Armin Fuchs,et al.  Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner—experimental evidence and theoretical model , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[45]  M. Kaur,et al.  Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony between children with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). , 2018, Research in developmental disabilities.

[46]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Evaluating the importance of social motor synchronization and motor skill for understanding autism , 2017, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[47]  Mary Beth Nebel,et al.  Intrinsic Visual-Motor Synchrony Correlates With Social Deficits in Autism , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[48]  T. Porkka-Heiskanen,et al.  Clinical neurological abnormalities in young adults with Asperger syndrome , 2006, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[49]  Line Garnero,et al.  Inter-Brain Synchronization during Social Interaction , 2010, PloS one.

[50]  A. Buja,et al.  Exploring the Relationship Between Anxiety and Insistence on Sameness in Autism Spectrum Disorders , 2013, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[51]  Gaël Varoquaux,et al.  The NumPy Array: A Structure for Efficient Numerical Computation , 2011, Computing in Science & Engineering.

[52]  Piercarlo Valdesolo,et al.  Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. , 2011, Emotion.

[53]  Mohamed Chetouani,et al.  Interpersonal Synchronization, Motor Coordination, and Control Are Impaired During a Dynamic Imitation Task in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[54]  T. Vos,et al.  The epidemiology and global burden of autism spectrum disorders , 2014, Psychological Medicine.

[55]  Jeffrey Eilbott,et al.  Cortical Activation during Action Observation, Action Execution, and Interpersonal Synchrony in Adults: A functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) Study , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[56]  Michael J. Richardson,et al.  Social Motor Synchronization: Insights for Understanding Social Behavior in Autism , 2017, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

[57]  A. Bhat,et al.  Differences in praxis performance and receptive language during fingerspelling between deaf children with and without autism spectrum disorder , 2018, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[58]  Dima Amso,et al.  Sensitivity to audio‐visual synchrony and its relation to language abilities in children with and without ASD , 2018, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[59]  J. Kelso,et al.  The human dynamic clamp as a paradigm for social interaction , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[60]  J. Reddon,et al.  Normative data stratified by age and sex for 12 neuropsychological tests , 1986 .

[61]  Joseph A. Bulbulia,et al.  To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect , 2017 .

[62]  K. Koelkebeck,et al.  Discriminating autism spectrum disorders from schizophrenia by investigation of mental state attribution on an on-line mentalizing task: A review and meta-analysis , 2016, Schizophrenia Research.

[63]  G. Baird,et al.  Impairment in movement skills of children with autistic spectrum disorders , 2009, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[64]  E. Ota,et al.  A systematic review and meta-analysis of comprehensive interventions for pre-school children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) , 2017, PloS one.

[65]  N. Rinehart,et al.  Are Motor Control and Regulation Problems Part of the ASD Motor Profile? A Handwriting Study , 2018, Developmental neuropsychology.

[66]  Jean Daunizeau,et al.  Social behavioural adaptation in Autism , 2020, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[67]  Dimitris Xygalatas,et al.  Sync to link: Endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation , 2017, Biological Psychology.

[68]  R. Kana,et al.  The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder , 2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[69]  Perception–action coupling and imitation in autism spectrum disorder , 2015, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[70]  G. Dawson,et al.  Implementing early intervention for autism spectrum disorder: a global perspective. , 2019, Pediatric medicine.

[71]  Inge-Marie Eigsti,et al.  Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency , 2010, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[72]  Leonhard Schilbach,et al.  Beyond one Bayesian brain: Modeling intra- and inter-personal processes during social interaction: Commentary on “Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference” by Fotopoulou & Tsakiris , 2017 .

[73]  I. Rezek,et al.  Understanding Heterogeneity in Clinical Cohorts Using Normative Models: Beyond Case-Control Studies , 2016, Biological Psychiatry.

[74]  Lori-Ann R. Sacrey,et al.  Early Motor Abilities in Infants at Heightened Versus Low Risk for ASD: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium (BSRC) Study , 2019, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[75]  Mark T. Wallace,et al.  Uncoupling Between Multisensory Temporal Function and Nonverbal Turn-Taking in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2018, IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems.

[76]  J. Tiffin,et al.  The Purdue pegboard; norms and studies of reliability and validity. , 1948, The Journal of applied psychology.

[77]  Joel Nothman,et al.  SciPy 1.0-Fundamental Algorithms for Scientific Computing in Python , 2019, ArXiv.

[78]  S. Rogers,et al.  Effects of Parent‐Implemented Early Start Denver Model Intervention on Chinese Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Non‐Randomized Controlled Trial , 2018, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[79]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Impairments of Social Motor Synchrony Evident in Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[80]  I. Hickie,et al.  Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function , 2017, Molecular Psychiatry.

[81]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  The Human Dynamic Clamp Reveals the Fronto-Parietal Network Linking Real-Time Social Coordination and Cognition , 2019, bioRxiv.

[82]  Z. Han,et al.  Autism Symptoms Modulate Interpersonal Neural Synchronization in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Cooperative Interactions , 2018, Brain Topography.

[83]  Gael Varoquaux,et al.  Establishment of Best Practices for Evidence for Prediction: A Review. , 2019, JAMA psychiatry.

[84]  Sonya Girdler,et al.  Standing Postural Control in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis , 2017, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[85]  E. Redcay,et al.  Interpersonal Synchrony in Autism , 2020, Current Psychiatry Reports.

[86]  Michael J Richardson,et al.  Evidence of embodied social competence during conversation in high functioning children with autism spectrum disorder , 2018, PloS one.

[87]  Pat Mirenda,et al.  Investigating phenotypic heterogeneity in children with autism spectrum disorder: a factor mixture modeling approach. , 2012, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[88]  J. A. Scott Kelso,et al.  The Virtual Teacher (VT) Paradigm: Learning New Patterns of Interpersonal Coordination Using the Human Dynamic Clamp , 2015, PloS one.