Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation

To accurately localize a visual target in space despite eye movement‐induced shifts of its retinal image, the brain must take into account both its retinal location and information about current eye position or at least the preceding eye displacement. We examined this ability with respect to saccadic eye movements by applying “double‐step” stimuli, where the locations of two sequentially flashed target lights have to be fixated by two successive saccades performed after their disappearance. As the 2nd saccade will not start at the spatial location from which the 2nd target was seen, a dissonance arises between its retinal coordinates and the motor coordinates of the required 2nd saccade. Nevertheless, these saccades were performed quite accurately by 32 healthy human adults. To investigate the contribution of the cerebral cortex, we recorded horizontal double‐step saccades in 35 patients with focal unilateral hemispheric lesions. Whereas frontal lesions impaired temporal properties, posterior parietal lesions caused spatial dysmetria or failure of even ipsiversive 2nd saccades following contraversive 1st saccades. This reflects an inability to compensate for retino‐spatial dissonance by using nonretinal information (corollary discharge) about eye displacement associated with a previous saccade into the contralesional hemifield. In conclusion, the parietal cortex is crucial for spatial constancy across saccades.

[1]  G. Westheimer Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. , 1954, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[2]  V. Mountcastle,et al.  Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. , 1975, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets , 1976, Vision Research.

[4]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[5]  D. Sparks,et al.  Dissociation of visual and saccade-related responses in superior colliculus neurons. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. , 1985, Science.

[8]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Timing function of the frontal cortex in sequential motor and learning tasks. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[9]  Barrett Katz,et al.  Binocular eye movement recording with CCD arrays , 1987 .

[10]  René Müri,et al.  Cortical control of saccades , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[11]  D. Kennedy,et al.  Magnetic resonance imaging–based brain morphometry: Development and application to normal subjects , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[12]  J. Lynch,et al.  Deficits of visual attention and saccadic eye movements after lesions of parietooccipital cortex in monkeys. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  E Wyllie,et al.  Versive eye movements elicited by cortical stimulation of the human brain , 1990, Neurology.

[15]  S. Rivaud,et al.  Impairment of sequences of memory‐guided saccades after supplementary motor area lesions , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[16]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Representation of visuomotor space in the parietal lobe of the monkey. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[17]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  The behavioural assessment of visual neglect , 1991 .

[19]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Neuronal activity related to saccadic eye movements in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccadic dysmetria in a patient with a right frontoparietal lesion. The importance of corollary discharge for accurate spatial behaviour. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  F Pedersen,et al.  Volume of Pituitary Macroadenomas: Assessment by MRI , 1992, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[22]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[23]  J. Stein The representation of egocentric space in the posterior parietal cortex. , 1992, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[24]  P Dassonville,et al.  Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[25]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions and oculomotor delayed-response performance: evidence for mnemonic "scotomas" , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  A. Berthoz,et al.  PET study of voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans: basal ganglia-thalamocortical system and cingulate cortex involvement. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Cortical control of saccades in man. , 1991, Acta neurologica Belgica.

[29]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[30]  J. Schlag,et al.  Supplementary eye field: influence of eye position on neural signals of fixation , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  W. A. Fletcher,et al.  Eye position signals in human saccadic processing , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  R. S. Gellman,et al.  Early responses to double-step targets are independent of step amplitude , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.