Feelings of shame and guilt are associated with distinct neural activation in youth

[1]  Shen Zhang,et al.  Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses , 2019, NeuroImage.

[2]  Nicholas B. Allen,et al.  Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective , 2018, Nature.

[3]  Christopher G. Davey,et al.  Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[4]  Christopher G. Davey,et al.  Mapping the self in the brain's default mode network , 2016, NeuroImage.

[5]  B. Harrison,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Neurodevelopmental Correlates of Proneness to Guilt and Shame in Adolescence and Early Adulthood , 2022 .

[6]  L. Uddin Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  Hongbo Yu,et al.  The voice of conscience: neural bases of interpersonal guilt and compensation. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[8]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Abnormal processing of deontological guilt in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[9]  D. Amodio,et al.  Neural correlates of experienced moral emotion: An fMRI investigation of emotion in response to prejudice feedback , 2014, Social neuroscience.

[10]  T. Meindl,et al.  Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: a pilot fMRI study. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[11]  Erdem Pulcu,et al.  Increased Amygdala Response to Shame in Remitted Major Depressive Disorder , 2014, PloS one.

[12]  Peter Zimmermann,et al.  Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood , 2014 .

[13]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Northoff,et al.  Reduced deactivation in reward circuitry and midline structures during emotion processing in borderline personality disorder , 2013, The world journal of biological psychiatry : the official journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry.

[15]  Kristen A. Lindquist,et al.  A functional architecture of the human brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Guilt-selective functional disconnection of anterior temporal and subgenual cortices in major depressive disorder. , 2012, Archives of general psychiatry.

[17]  B. Harrison,et al.  Neural correlates of moral sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2012, Archives of general psychiatry.

[18]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Neural activity associated with self-reflection , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.

[19]  Gregory McCarthy,et al.  Neural systems for guilt from actions affecting self versus others , 2012, NeuroImage.

[20]  Debra Mashek,et al.  Assessing Jail Inmates’ Proneness To Shame and Guilt , 2011, Criminal justice and behavior.

[21]  U. Wagner,et al.  Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  D. Sharp,et al.  Fractionating the Default Mode Network: Distinct Contributions of the Ventral and Dorsal Posterior Cingulate Cortex to Cognitive Control , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Katie A McLaughlin,et al.  Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.

[24]  T. Egner,et al.  Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Deontological and altruistic guilt: Evidence for distinct neurobiological substrates , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Jordan Henry Grafman,et al.  Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia , 2011, NeuroImage.

[27]  Emily L. Dennis,et al.  Neural correlates of rumination in depression , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Selective functional integration between anterior temporal and distinct fronto-mesolimbic regions during guilt and indignation , 2010, NeuroImage.

[29]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[30]  Najmeh Khalili-Mahani,et al.  Hippocampal activation during a cognitive task is associated with subsequent neuroendocrine and cognitive responses to psychological stress , 2009, Hippocampus.

[31]  André Aleman,et al.  Activation of Anterior Insula during Self-Reflection , 2009, PloS one.

[32]  B. Harrison,et al.  Posterior cingulate activation during moral dilemma in adolescents , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[33]  B. Harrison,et al.  Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  G. Fink,et al.  Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[35]  J. Tangney,et al.  Moral emotions and moral behavior. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[36]  J. Decety,et al.  The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[37]  Marcel Zeelenberg,et al.  Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt , 2007 .

[38]  I. Olson,et al.  The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[40]  Nathalie Boddaert,et al.  Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[41]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[42]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[43]  J. Decety,et al.  The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior , 2006, Brain Research.

[44]  A. Cavanna,et al.  The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  H. Heinze,et al.  Reward-Related fMRI Activation of Dopaminergic Midbrain Is Associated with Enhanced Hippocampus- Dependent Long-Term Memory Formation , 2005, Neuron.

[46]  R. Habib,et al.  Activation of midbrain structures by associative novelty and the formation of explicit memory in humans. , 2004, Learning & memory.

[47]  Jessica L. Tracy,et al.  Putting the Self into Self-conscious Emotions: a Theoretical Model Distinctive Features of Self-conscious Emotions Self-conscious Emotions Require Self-awareness and Self-representations Self-conscious Emotions Emerge Later in Childhood than Basic Emotions Self-conscious Emotions Are Cognitively Com , 2022 .

[48]  L. Davachi,et al.  Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[49]  M. Raichle,et al.  Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[51]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  T. Ferguson,et al.  Unwanted Identities: A Key Variable in Shame–Anger Links and Gender Differences in Shame , 2000 .

[53]  R. Dolan,et al.  Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  J. Desmond,et al.  Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. , 1998, Science.

[55]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  Empathy, Shame, Guilt, and Narratives of Interpersonal Conflicts: Guilt‐Prone People Are Better at Perspective Taking , 1998 .

[56]  T. Ferguson,et al.  Gender Differences in the Organization of Guilt and Shame , 1997 .

[57]  R. S. Miller,et al.  Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[59]  J. Tangney,et al.  Recent Advances in the Empirical Study of Shame and Guilt , 1995 .

[60]  B. Andrews Bodily shame as a mediator between abusive experiences and depression. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[61]  Karen Caplovitz Barrett,et al.  Avoiders vs. Amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during Toddlerhood? , 1993 .

[62]  J. Tangney,et al.  Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. , 1992, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[63]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[64]  J. Tsai,et al.  Cultural models of shame and guilt. , 2007 .

[65]  J. Smetana,et al.  Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.

[66]  N. Eisenberg Emotion, regulation, and moral development. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[67]  D. Keltner,et al.  The forms and functions of the nonverbal signal of shame. , 1998 .