Feelings of shame and guilt are associated with distinct neural activation in youth
暂无分享,去创建一个
B. Harrison | N. Allen | C. Davey | S. Whittle | C. Bastin | Divyangana Rakesh | S. Muller
[1] Shen Zhang,et al. Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses , 2019, NeuroImage.
[2] Nicholas B. Allen,et al. Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective , 2018, Nature.
[3] Christopher G. Davey,et al. Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[4] Christopher G. Davey,et al. Mapping the self in the brain's default mode network , 2016, NeuroImage.
[5] B. Harrison,et al. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Neurodevelopmental Correlates of Proneness to Guilt and Shame in Adolescence and Early Adulthood , 2022 .
[6] L. Uddin. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction , 2014, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[7] Hongbo Yu,et al. The voice of conscience: neural bases of interpersonal guilt and compensation. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[8] E. Macaluso,et al. Abnormal processing of deontological guilt in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.
[9] D. Amodio,et al. Neural correlates of experienced moral emotion: An fMRI investigation of emotion in response to prejudice feedback , 2014, Social neuroscience.
[10] T. Meindl,et al. Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: a pilot fMRI study. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.
[11] Erdem Pulcu,et al. Increased Amygdala Response to Shame in Remitted Major Depressive Disorder , 2014, PloS one.
[12] Peter Zimmermann,et al. Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood , 2014 .
[13] Brian A. Nosek,et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[14] G. Northoff,et al. Reduced deactivation in reward circuitry and midline structures during emotion processing in borderline personality disorder , 2013, The world journal of biological psychiatry : the official journal of the World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry.
[15] Kristen A. Lindquist,et al. A functional architecture of the human brain: emerging insights from the science of emotion , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[16] M. L. Lambon Ralph,et al. Guilt-selective functional disconnection of anterior temporal and subgenual cortices in major depressive disorder. , 2012, Archives of general psychiatry.
[17] B. Harrison,et al. Neural correlates of moral sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2012, Archives of general psychiatry.
[18] L. Jäncke,et al. Neural activity associated with self-reflection , 2012, BMC Neuroscience.
[19] Gregory McCarthy,et al. Neural systems for guilt from actions affecting self versus others , 2012, NeuroImage.
[20] Debra Mashek,et al. Assessing Jail Inmates’ Proneness To Shame and Guilt , 2011, Criminal justice and behavior.
[21] U. Wagner,et al. Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.
[22] D. Sharp,et al. Fractionating the Default Mode Network: Distinct Contributions of the Ventral and Dorsal Posterior Cingulate Cortex to Cognitive Control , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] Katie A McLaughlin,et al. Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.
[24] T. Egner,et al. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[25] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Deontological and altruistic guilt: Evidence for distinct neurobiological substrates , 2011, Human brain mapping.
[26] Jordan Henry Grafman,et al. Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia , 2011, NeuroImage.
[27] Emily L. Dennis,et al. Neural correlates of rumination in depression , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[28] Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al. Selective functional integration between anterior temporal and distinct fronto-mesolimbic regions during guilt and indignation , 2010, NeuroImage.
[29] V. Menon,et al. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.
[30] Najmeh Khalili-Mahani,et al. Hippocampal activation during a cognitive task is associated with subsequent neuroendocrine and cognitive responses to psychological stress , 2009, Hippocampus.
[31] André Aleman,et al. Activation of Anterior Insula during Self-Reflection , 2009, PloS one.
[32] B. Harrison,et al. Posterior cingulate activation during moral dilemma in adolescents , 2008, Human brain mapping.
[33] B. Harrison,et al. Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[34] G. Fink,et al. Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[35] J. Tangney,et al. Moral emotions and moral behavior. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[36] J. Decety,et al. The Role of the Right Temporoparietal Junction in Social Interaction: How Low-Level Computational Processes Contribute to Meta-Cognition , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[37] Marcel Zeelenberg,et al. Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt , 2007 .
[38] I. Olson,et al. The Enigmatic temporal pole: a review of findings on social and emotional processing. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[39] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.
[40] Nathalie Boddaert,et al. Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.
[41] Jason P. Mitchell,et al. Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.
[42] Georg Northoff,et al. Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.
[43] J. Decety,et al. The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's behavior , 2006, Brain Research.
[44] A. Cavanna,et al. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[45] H. Heinze,et al. Reward-Related fMRI Activation of Dopaminergic Midbrain Is Associated with Enhanced Hippocampus- Dependent Long-Term Memory Formation , 2005, Neuron.
[46] R. Habib,et al. Activation of midbrain structures by associative novelty and the formation of explicit memory in humans. , 2004, Learning & memory.
[47] Jessica L. Tracy,et al. Putting the Self into Self-conscious Emotions: a Theoretical Model Distinctive Features of Self-conscious Emotions Self-conscious Emotions Require Self-awareness and Self-representations Self-conscious Emotions Emerge Later in Childhood than Basic Emotions Self-conscious Emotions Are Cognitively Com , 2022 .
[48] L. Davachi,et al. Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: insights from relational and item-based learning. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[49] M. Raichle,et al. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[50] G L Shulman,et al. INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .
[51] Karl J. Friston,et al. Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[52] T. Ferguson,et al. Unwanted Identities: A Key Variable in Shame–Anger Links and Gender Differences in Shame , 2000 .
[53] R. Dolan,et al. Common effects of emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation during visual processing of pictures , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[54] J. Desmond,et al. Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. , 1998, Science.
[55] Roy F. Baumeister,et al. Empathy, Shame, Guilt, and Narratives of Interpersonal Conflicts: Guilt‐Prone People Are Better at Perspective Taking , 1998 .
[56] T. Ferguson,et al. Gender Differences in the Organization of Guilt and Shame , 1997 .
[57] R. S. Miller,et al. Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[58] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.
[59] J. Tangney,et al. Recent Advances in the Empirical Study of Shame and Guilt , 1995 .
[60] B. Andrews. Bodily shame as a mediator between abusive experiences and depression. , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[61] Karen Caplovitz Barrett,et al. Avoiders vs. Amenders: Implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during Toddlerhood? , 1993 .
[62] J. Tangney,et al. Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. , 1992, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[63] A. Craig,et al. How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[64] J. Tsai,et al. Cultural models of shame and guilt. , 2007 .
[65] J. Smetana,et al. Adolescent development in interpersonal and societal contexts. , 2006, Annual review of psychology.
[66] N. Eisenberg. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.
[67] D. Keltner,et al. The forms and functions of the nonverbal signal of shame. , 1998 .