Effects of sustained, voluntary attention on amplitude and latency of steady-state visual evoked potential: a costs and benefits analysis
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Vogel,et al. Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[2] D. Spinelli,et al. Spatial attention has different effects on the magno- and parvocellular pathways. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[3] D. Regan. Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine , 1989 .
[4] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28 Hz range. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[5] J D Victor,et al. How the contrast gain control modifies the frequency responses of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.
[6] M. Posner. Attention in cognitive neuroscience: An overview. , 1995 .
[7] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.
[8] S. Hillyard,et al. Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[9] S. Hillyard,et al. Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[10] David C. Burr,et al. The effects of ageing on the pattern electroretinogram and visual evoked potential in humans , 1991, Vision Research.
[11] C. Koch,et al. Attention activates winner-take-all competition among visual filters , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[12] S. Hillyard,et al. Selective attention to stimulus location modulates the steady-state visual evoked potential. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[13] S A Hillyard,et al. Sensory gating as a physiological mechanism for visual selective attention. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[14] S J Luck,et al. Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] B. Knight,et al. Contrast gain control in the primate retina: P cells are not X-like, some M cells are , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[16] Steven A. Hillyard,et al. Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. , 1994 .
[17] Richard B Buxton,et al. Putting spatial attention on the map: timing and localization of stimulus selection processes in striate and extrastriate visual areas , 2001, Vision Research.
[18] M C Morrone,et al. Spatial neglect is associated with increased latencies of visual evoked potentials , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.
[19] F. Di Russo,et al. Electrophysiological analysis of cortical mechanisms of selective attention to high and low spatial frequencies , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[20] D. Burr,et al. Development of the Temporal Properties of Visual Evoked Potentials to Luminance and Colour Contrast in Infants , 1996, Vision Research.
[21] M. C. Morrone,et al. Cross-orientation inhibition in cat is GABA mediated , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[22] F. Campbell,et al. The effect of chromatic aberration on visual acuity , 1967, The Journal of physiology.
[23] M. Posner,et al. Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.
[24] Matthias M. Müller,et al. Concurrent recording of steady-state and transient event-related potentials as indices of visual-spatial selective attention , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[25] M. Eimer. ERP modulations indicate the selective processing of visual stimuli as a result of transient and sustained spatial attention. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[26] R. Shapley,et al. The effect of contrast on the transfer properties of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.
[27] D. Regan. Some characteristics of average steady-state and transient responses evoked by modulated light. , 1966, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[28] M. Carandini,et al. Summation and division by neurons in primate visual cortex. , 1994, Science.
[29] George R. Mangun,et al. Sustained visual-spatial attention produces costs and benefits in response time and evoked neural activity , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[30] B McElree,et al. Covert attention accelerates the rate of visual information processing , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[31] Matthias M. Müller,et al. The time course of cortical facilitation during cued shifts of spatial attention , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] G. Mangun,et al. Electrophysiological and behavioral "costs" and "benefits" during sustained visual-spatial attention. , 1994, The International journal of neuroscience.
[33] E. Vogel,et al. The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[34] D. Burr,et al. Development of infant contrast sensitivity to chromatic stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.
[35] Donatella Spinelli,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for an early attentional mechanism in visual processing in humans , 1999, Vision Research.
[36] J. Kelsey,et al. Visual Evoked Potentials in Man: New Developments , 1978 .
[37] F. Campbell,et al. Electrophysiological evidence for the existence of orientation and size detectors in the human visual system , 1970, The Journal of physiology.
[38] E. Bizzi,et al. The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .