Non-Symbolic Exact Quantity Representation in a Language Impaired Population

English-speakers whose access to number language is artificially compromised by verbal interference and the Pirahã (an Amazonian tribe without exact number words) appear to rely on analog magnitude estimation for representing nonsymbolic exact quantities greater than 3. Here, 16 participants with aphasia performed the 5 counting tasks from these previous studies. Performance was poorest when targets were not visible during response (70% correct, task 4; 71% correct, task 5) and best when targets were presented as subitizable groups of 2 and 3 (98% correct, task 2). Western Aphasia Battery-Revised subtest scores correlated with task performance, suggesting diverse forms of language impairment may contribute to errors. Coefficients of variation for tasks and significant correlations of target magnitude with error rate (r=.88) and error size (r=.87) across tasks suggest participant use of analog magnitude estimation. Experiments involving people with aphasia may further refine our understanding of how language and thought interact.

[1]  M. McNeil,et al.  Defining aphasia: Some theoretical and clinical implications of operating from a formal definition , 2001 .

[2]  Robert T. Wertz,et al.  Aphasia: A Clinical Approach , 1989 .

[3]  Randi C. Martin,et al.  Dissociable components of short-term memory and their relation to long-term learning , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[4]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[5]  Caleb Everett,et al.  Quantity Recognition Among Speakers of an Anumeric Language , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[6]  M. Weisz Language In Mind Advances In The Study Of Language And Thought , 2016 .

[7]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[8]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  The development of language and abstract concepts: the case of natural number. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[9]  C. Everett Linguistic Relativity: Evidence Across Languages and Cognitive Domains , 2013 .

[10]  Mario A. Pei,et al.  Language, Thought and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf , 1957 .

[11]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Approximate quantities and exact number words: dissociable systems , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[13]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Verbal interference suppresses exact numerical representation , 2011, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  P. Gordon Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia , 2004, Science.

[15]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition , 2008, Cognition.

[16]  E. Spelke,et al.  Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  N. Dronkers,et al.  Toward a functional neuroanatomy of semantic aphasia: A history and ten new cases , 2017, Cortex.

[18]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[19]  Παρασκευή Ελένη Κουρκούλη Western Aphasia Battery Revised. Μια πιλοτική εφαρμογή του WAB-R σε μη παθολογικό πληθυσμό και σε ασθενείς με άνοια , 2014 .

[20]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[21]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Cultural Differences in Perceptual Reorganization in US and Pirahã Adults , 2014, PloS one.

[23]  B. L. Whorf Language, Thought, and Reality: Selected Writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf , 1956 .