Visual working memory for simple and complex features: An fMRI study

Visual working memory (VWM) allows us to hold visual information briefly in our minds after its disappearance. It is important for bridging the present to the immediate past. Previous neuroscience studies on VWM have shown that several parietal, frontal, and occipitotemporal brain regions subserve this function. Those studies, however, have often focused on VWM for a single property, such as color. Yet, in behavior, the capacity of VWM is sensitive to the complexity of to-be-remembered visual features. How do different brain areas represent VWM for visual features of different complexity and for combination of features? To address this question, we used functional MRI to study the response profile of several brain regions in three VWM tasks. In all tasks, subjects saw 1 to 7 colored polygons and had to remember their color (a simple feature), shape (a complex feature), or both color and shape. Behavioral performance showed that VWM reached capacity limit at about 3 colors, 2 shapes, and 2 compound objects. In the fMRI data, we found different functional profiles for frontal, parietal, and occipitotemporal regions. Specifically, the posterior parietal cortex was sensitive to both featural and VWM load manipulations. The prefrontal regions were sensitive to VWM load manipulation but relatively insensitive to featural differences. The occipitotemporal regions were sensitive to featural differences, but not to VWM load manipulation. We propose that the response properties of these regions can jointly account for several findings in human VWM behavior.

[1]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  Robert H. Logie,et al.  Visuo-spatial short-term memory: Visual Working Memory or Visual Buffer? , 1991 .

[3]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  E. Vogel,et al.  Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004 .

[5]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Cortical Regions Involved in Perceiving Object Shape , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  J M Fuster,et al.  Behavioral electrophysiology of the prefrontal cortex of the primate. , 1990, Progress in brain research.

[7]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Directing the mind's eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[8]  Gary H Glover,et al.  Estimating sample size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging studies: Statistical power analyses , 2002, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[9]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural Correlates of Visual Working Memory fMRI Amplitude Predicts Task Performance , 2002, Neuron.

[10]  R. Marois,et al.  Capacity limits of information processing in the brain , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  Goldman-Rakic Ps Cellular and circuit basis of working memory in prefrontal cortex of nonhuman primates. , 1990 .

[12]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Yuhong V. Jiang,et al.  Is visual short-term memory object based? Rejection of the “strong-object” hypothesis , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[14]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  The Capacity of Visual Short-Term Memory is Set Both by Visual Information Load and by Number of Objects , 2004, Psychological science.

[15]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[16]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. , 1998, Science.

[17]  R. Desimone,et al.  Parallel neuronal mechanisms for short-term memory. , 1994, Science.

[18]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Activity in fusiform face area modulated as a function of working memory load. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[19]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  A neural system for human visual working memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  B. Postle,et al.  “ What ” — Then — “ Where ” in Visual Working Memory : An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2000 .

[21]  W. Singer,et al.  Distributed cortical systems in visual short-term memory revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  M. Chun,et al.  Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects , 2006, Nature.

[23]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[24]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[26]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Working Memory in Another Dimension: Functional Imaging of Human Olfactory Working Memory , 2001, NeuroImage.

[27]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Attention Response Functions Characterizing Brain Areas Using fMRI Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load , 2001, Neuron.

[28]  P. Pala,et al.  Domain interactions of H–2 class I antigens alter cytotoxic T-cell recognition sites , 1984, Nature.

[29]  M. Chun,et al.  Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  C. Koch,et al.  Brain Areas Specific for Attentional Load in a Motion-Tracking Task , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  S C Rao,et al.  Integration of what and where in the primate prefrontal cortex. , 1997, Science.

[32]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  The human visual cortex. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Dissecting Contributions of Prefrontal Cortex and Fusiform Face Area to Face Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Eye position effects on visual, memory, and saccade-related activity in areas LIP and 7a of macaque , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Temporal isolation of the neural correlates of spatial mnemonic processing with fMRI. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Memory for object-features versus memory for object-location: A positron emission tomography study of encoding and retrieval processes , 1996, NeuroImage.

[37]  A. Treisman,et al.  Binding in short-term visual memory. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[38]  R. Desimone,et al.  Object and place memory in the macaque entorhinal cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[40]  G. McCarthy,et al.  The Influence of Memory Load Upon Delay-Interval Activity in a Working-Memory Task: An Event-Related Functional MRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  B R Postle,et al.  "What"-Then-Where" in visual working memory: an event-related fMRI study. , 1999, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.