Directing attention to locations and to sensory modalities: multiple levels of selective processing revealed with PET.

We used positron emission tomography (PET) to investigate the neural correlates of selective attention in humans. We examined the effects of attending to one side of space versus another (spatial selection) and to one sensory modality versus another (intermodal selection) during bilateral, bimodal stimulation of vision and touch. Attention toward one side resulted in greater activity in several contralateral areas. In somatosensory cortex, these spatial attentional modulations were found only when touch was relevant. In the intraparietal sulcus, spatial attentional effects were multimodal, independent of the modality attended. In occipital areas, spatial modulations were also found during both visual and tactile attention, indicating that tactile attention can affect activity in visual cortex; but occipital areas also showed more activity overall during visual attention. This suggests that while spatial attention can exert multimodal influences on visual areas, these still maintain their specificity for the visual modality. Additionally, irrespective of the attended side, attending to vision activated posterior parietal and superior premotor cortices, while attending to touch activated the parietal operculi. We conclude that attentional selection operates at multiple levels, with attention to locations and attention to modalities showing distinct effects. These jointly contribute to boost processing of stimuli at the attended location in the relevant modality.

[1]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[2]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in a polysensory area in superior temporal sulcus of the macaque. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  B. C. Motter,et al.  The functional properties of the light-sensitive neurons of the posterior parietal cortex studied in waking monkeys: foveal sparing and opponent vector organization , 1981, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  P. Roland Cortical regulation of selective attention in man. A regional cerebral blood flow study. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  A Noninvasive Positron Computed Tomography Technique Using Oxygen-15-Labeled Water for the Evaluation of Neurobehavioral Task Batteries , 1985, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[6]  Charles M. Butter,et al.  Impairments in orienting to visual stimuli in monkeys following unilateral lesions of the superior sulcal polysensory cortex , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  D. Perani,et al.  The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man , 1986, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  E. G. Jones Cerebral Cortex , 1987, Cerebral Cortex.

[9]  M. Raichle,et al.  Mapping human somatosensory cortex with positron emission tomography. , 1987, Journal of neurosurgery.

[10]  M. Mintun,et al.  Noninvasive functional brain mapping by change-distribution analysis of averaged PET images of H215O tissue activity. , 1989, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[11]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Combined spatial and temporal imaging of brain activity during visual selective attention in humans , 1994, Nature.

[14]  K. Heilman,et al.  Posterior neocortical systems subserving awareness and neglect. Neglect associated with superior temporal sulcus but not area 7 lesions. , 1994, Archives of neurology.

[15]  D. Collins,et al.  Automatic 3D Intersubject Registration of MR Volumetric Data in Standardized Talairach Space , 1994, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[16]  M. Raichle,et al.  Blood flow changes in human somatosensory cortex during anticipated stimulation , 1995, Nature.

[17]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[19]  J. Driver,et al.  Audiovisual links in endogenous covert spatial attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The Role of the Thalamus in “Top Down” Modulation of Attention to Sound , 1996, NeuroImage.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[22]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  G A Orban,et al.  Attention to One or Two Features in Left or Right Visual Field: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  J V Haxby,et al.  Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  B. Renault,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Human Auditory Attention Studied with PET , 1997, NeuroImage.

[26]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[29]  E. Schröger,et al.  ERP effects of intermodal attention and cross-modal links in spatial attention. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[30]  Hiroshi Shibasaki,et al.  Attention modulates both primary and second somatosensory cortical activities in humans: a magnetoencephalographic study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  F. J. Friedrich,et al.  Spatial attention deficits in humans: a comparison of superior parietal and temporal-parietal junction lesions. , 1998, Neuropsychology.

[32]  E. DeYoe,et al.  A physiological correlate of the 'spotlight' of visual attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[34]  R. Hari,et al.  Relationship between Responses to Contra- and Ipsilateral Stimuli in the Human Second Somatosensory Cortex SII , 1999, NeuroImage.

[35]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Selective tuning of the left and right auditory cortices during spatially directed attention. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[36]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Can attention be directed to opposite locations in different modalities? An ERP study , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[37]  H Burton,et al.  Tactile attention tasks enhance activation in somatosensory regions of parietal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Selective Visual and Auditory Attention Toward Utterances—A PET Study , 1999, NeuroImage.

[39]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[41]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  J Driver,et al.  Cross-modal selective attention: On the difficulty of ignoring sounds at the locus of visual attention , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  C. Spence,et al.  Crossmodal links between vision and touch in covert endogenous spatial attention. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Erratum to “Translocation machinery for synthesis of integral membrane and secretory proteins in dendritic spines” , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  J Driver,et al.  Selective spatial attention in vision and touch: unimodal and multimodal mechanisms revealed by PET. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Frith,et al.  Interhemispheric Differences in Extrastriate Areas during Visuo-Spatial Selective Attention , 2000, NeuroImage.

[48]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Neurobiological measures of human selective attention , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  Charles Spence Crossmodal attention , 1998, Scholarpedia.