Linguistic distance dynamically modulates the effects of bilingualism on executive performance in aging
暂无分享,去创建一个
A. Myachykov | J. Abutalebi | F. Gallo | L. Terekhina | Yury Shtyrov | Maria Nelyubina | Joanna Kubiak
[1] J. Abutalebi,et al. The unique role of bilingualism among cognitive reserve-enhancing factors , 2023, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition.
[2] J. Rothman,et al. Bilingualism and Aging: Implications for (Delaying) Neurocognitive Decline , 2022, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
[3] A. Myachykov,et al. Add Bilingualism to the Mix: L2 Proficiency Modulates the Effect of Cognitive Reserve Proxies on Executive Performance in Healthy Aging , 2022, Frontiers in Psychology.
[4] L. Fratiglioni,et al. Cognitive Trajectories and Dementia Risk: A Comparison of Two Cognitive Reserve Measures , 2021, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience.
[5] E. Bialystok. Bilingualism: Pathway to Cognitive Reserve , 2021, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[6] L. Fratiglioni,et al. Education and Cognitive Functioning Across the Life Span , 2020, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.
[7] E. Blom,et al. Cross-language distance influences receptive vocabulary outcomes of bilingual children , 2020 .
[8] D. E. Sörman,et al. The bilingual effects of linguistic distances on episodic memory and verbal fluency. , 2019, Scandinavian journal of psychology.
[9] Y. Joanette,et al. Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Control: The Effect of Bilingualism and Age on the Subcomponents of Attention , 2019, Front. Neurol..
[10] J. Abutalebi,et al. Second language use rather than second language knowledge relates to changes in white matter microstructure , 2019, Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science.
[11] M. Lövdén,et al. Education and age-related decline in cognitive performance: Systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies , 2019, Ageing Research Reviews.
[12] Hui-Xin Wang,et al. Genetic risk of dementia mitigated by cognitive reserve: A cohort study , 2019, Annals of neurology.
[13] Sabrina Aristei,et al. The Closer They Are, the More They Interfere: Semantic Similarity of Word Distractors Increases Competition in Language Production , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[14] Jason Rothman,et al. Redefining bilingualism as a spectrum of experiences that differentially affects brain structure and function , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[15] Patrik Hansson,et al. Different Features of Bilingualism in Relation to Executive Functioning , 2019, Front. Psychol..
[16] Y. Stern,et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance , 2018, Alzheimer's & Dementia.
[17] Isabell Wartenburger,et al. A close call: Interference from semantic neighbourhood density and similarity in language production , 2018, Memory & Cognition.
[18] Simone Sulpizio,et al. Neuroplasticity across the lifespan and aging effects in bilinguals and monolinguals , 2018, Brain and Cognition.
[19] M. Lövdén,et al. Education Does Not Affect Cognitive Decline in Aging: A Bayesian Assessment of the Association Between Education and Change in Cognitive Performance , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[20] N. Egorova,et al. Beyond bilingualism: multilingual experience correlates with caudate volume , 2017, Brain Structure and Function.
[21] E. Bialystok. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. , 2017, Psychological bulletin.
[22] G. Stoet. PsyToolkit , 2017 .
[23] Sheung-Tak Cheng. Cognitive Reserve and the Prevention of Dementia: the Role of Physical and Cognitive Activities , 2016, Current Psychiatry Reports.
[24] K. Paap,et al. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances , 2015, Cortex.
[25] Cristina Guardiano,et al. Across language families: Genome diversity mirrors linguistic variation within Europe , 2015, American journal of physical anthropology.
[26] Paola E. Dussias,et al. Bilingualism, Mind, and Brain. , 2015, Annual review of linguistics.
[27] M. Wilbourn,et al. Better early or late? Examining the influence of age of exposure and language proficiency on executive function in early and late bilinguals , 2014 .
[28] Walter J. B. van Heuven,et al. The effect of script similarity on executive control in bilinguals , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[29] J. García-García,et al. Estimating Cognitive Reserve in Healthy Adults Using the Cognitive Reserve Scale , 2014, PloS one.
[30] E. Thomas,et al. Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[31] E. Bialystok,et al. Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.
[32] D. Mapelli,et al. Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve , 2013, Aging Clinical and Experimental Research.
[33] D. Green,et al. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis , 2013, Journal of cognitive psychology.
[34] A. Vallesi,et al. Conflict resolution and adaptation in normal aging: the role of verbal intelligence and cognitive reserve. , 2012, Psychology and aging.
[35] Cary S Feria,et al. The Effects of Distractors in Multiple Object Tracking are Modulated by the Similarity of Distractor and Target Features , 2012, Perception.
[36] E. Bialystok,et al. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: role of language, cultural background, and education. , 2012, Child development.
[37] D. Salmon,et al. Cross-Language Intrusion Errors in Aging Bilinguals Reveal the Link Between Executive Control and Language Selection , 2011, Psychological science.
[38] R. Baayen,et al. Analyzing Reaction Times , 2010 .
[39] John A. E. Anderson,et al. Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression , 2010, Brain and Cognition.
[40] Eric W. Holman,et al. Evaluating linguistic distance measures , 2010 .
[41] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. Similarity Effect and Optimal Control of Multiple-Choice Decision Making , 2008, Neuron.
[42] R. Baayen,et al. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items , 2008 .
[43] Albert Costa,et al. Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task , 2008, Cognition.
[44] M. Serva,et al. Indo-European languages tree by Levenshtein distance , 2007, 0708.2971.
[45] Jubin Abutalebi,et al. Bilingual language production: The neurocognition of language representation and control , 2007, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[46] Ping Li,et al. The emergence of competing modules in bilingualism , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[47] Ellen Bialystok,et al. Bilingualism, Biliteracy, and Learning to Read: Interactions Among Languages and Writing Systems , 2005 .
[48] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[49] Albert Costa,et al. Lexical Selection in Bilinguals: Do Words in the Bilingual's Two Lexicons Compete for Selection? , 1999 .
[50] Noriko Hoshino,et al. Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. , 2008, The mental lexicon.