Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Enhances Verbal Working Memory Training Performance over Time and Near Transfer Outcomes

Studies attempting to increase working memory (WM) capacity show promise in enhancing related cognitive functions but have also raised criticism in the broader scientific community given the inconsistent findings produced by these studies. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been shown to enhance WM performance in a single session [Fregni, F., Boggio, P., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Anatal, A., Feredoes, E., et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 166, 23–30, 2005]; however, the extent to which tDCS might enhance learning on a WM training regime and the extent to which learning gains might transfer outside the training task remains largely unknown. To this end, participants engaged in an adaptive WM training task [previously utilized in Richmond, L., Morrison, A., Chein, J., & Olson, I. Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychology & Aging, 26, 813–822, 2011; Chein, J., & Morrison, A. Expanding the mind's workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 193–199, 2010] for 10 sessions over 2 weeks, concurrent with either active or sham stimulation of dorsolateral pFC. Before and after training, a battery of tests tapping domains known to relate to WM abilities was administered. Results show that tDCS enhanced learning on the verbal portion of the training task by 3.65 items. Furthermore, tDCS was shown to enhance near transfer to other untrained WM tasks in comparison with a no-contact control group. These results lend support to the idea that tDCS might bolster training and transfer gains in populations with compromised WM abilities.

[1]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consensus: "Can tDCS and TMS enhance motor learning and memory formation?" , 2008, Brain stimulation.

[2]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Jan Born,et al.  Bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation slows reaction time in a working memory task , 2005, BMC Neuroscience.

[4]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .

[5]  M. Nitsche,et al.  Facilitation of Implicit Motor Learning by Weak Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of the Primary Motor Cortex in the Human , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Lauren L. Richmond,et al.  Repeated Daily Exposure to Direct Current Stimulation Does Not Result in Sustained or Notable Side Effects , 2013, Brain Stimulation.

[7]  E. Koster,et al.  Combining tDCS and Working Memory Training to Down Regulate State Rumination: A Single-Session Double Blind Sham-Controlled Trial , 2015, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[8]  Alexandra B. Morrison,et al.  Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[9]  Melissa J. Green,et al.  Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance outcomes from cognitive training? A randomized controlled trial in healthy participants. , 2013, The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology.

[10]  J. Thorne,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence , 2011, BMC Neuroscience.

[11]  Julie M. Baker Using transcranial direct current stimulation to treat aphasia , 2009 .

[12]  L. Cohen,et al.  Efficacy of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation/transcranial direct current stimulation in cognitive neurorehabilitation , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[13]  C. Loo,et al.  A systematic review of transcranial electrical stimulation combined with cognitive training. , 2015, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[14]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with Parkinson's disease , 2006, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[15]  M. Sarter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[16]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  Is working memory training effective? , 2012, Psychological bulletin.

[17]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  N. Unsworth,et al.  Working memory capacity: Attention control, secondary memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model , 2010 .

[19]  R. Engle,et al.  The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. , 2007, Psychological review.

[20]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  Does working memory training generalize , 2010 .

[21]  Stefan Knecht,et al.  Electrical Stimulation of Broca's Area Enhances Implicit Learning of an Artificial Grammar , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[23]  N. Birbaumer,et al.  Enhancement of Planning Ability by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  James I. Brown The Nelson-Denny Reading Test. , 1960 .

[25]  Y. Kim,et al.  Improvement of the Working Memory by Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation in Healthy Older Adults , 2011 .

[26]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  The Relationship between N-Back Performance and Matrix Reasoning--Implications for Training and Transfer. , 2010 .

[27]  N. Chiaravalloti,et al.  Cognitive contributions to differences in learning after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury , 2015, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[28]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis , 2008, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[29]  C. Juan,et al.  Blending transcranial direct current stimulations and physical exercise to maximize cognitive improvement , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[30]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Age Differences in the Frontal Lateralization of Verbal and Spatial Working Memory Revealed by PET , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  T. Makovski,et al.  Stimulating occipital cortex enhances visual working memory consolidation , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  Susanne M. Jaeggi,et al.  Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[33]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Putting brain training to the test , 2010, Nature.

[34]  Luc Goossens,et al.  Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[35]  Sergio P. Rigonatti,et al.  Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[36]  Alvaro Pascual-Leone,et al.  Suppression of ipsilateral motor cortex facilitates motor skill learning , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .

[38]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  An automated version of the operation span task , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[39]  V. Walsh,et al.  Modulating behavioral inhibition by tDCS combined with cognitive training , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Ingrid R Olson,et al.  Working memory training and transfer in older adults. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[41]  R. Roberts,et al.  Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task. , 1994 .

[42]  Myoung-Hwan Ko,et al.  Enhancing the Working Memory of Stroke Patients Using tDCS , 2009, American journal of physical medicine & rehabilitation.

[43]  Alexandra B. Morrison,et al.  Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[44]  R. Zatorre,et al.  Polarity-specific transcranial direct current stimulation disrupts auditory pitch learning , 2015, Front. Neurosci..

[45]  Sergio P. Rigonatti,et al.  Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  S. Knecht,et al.  Non-invasive brain stimulation improves object-location learning in the elderly , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[47]  Heidi M. Schambra,et al.  Direct Current Stimulation Promotes BDNF-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity: Potential Implications for Motor Learning , 2010, Neuron.

[48]  R. Engle,et al.  The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  O. Carter,et al.  Quantitative Review Finds No Evidence of Cognitive Effects in Healthy Populations From Single-session Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) , 2015, Brain Stimulation.

[50]  T. Sanger,et al.  Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  Thomas S. Redick,et al.  No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  J. Krakauer,et al.  Consensus: Can transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial magnetic stimulation enhance motor learning and memory formation? , 2008, Brain Stimulation.

[53]  R. Engle,et al.  Executive Attention, Working Memory Capacity, and a Two-Factor Theory of Cognitive Control. , 2003 .

[54]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation? , 2009, Brain Stimulation.

[55]  Patrick C. Kyllonen,et al.  Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! , 1990 .

[56]  Michael F. Bunting,et al.  Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user’s guide , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[57]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[59]  V. Walsh,et al.  Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex modulates declarative memory , 2012, Brain Stimulation.

[60]  Mm. Liabre Standard Progressive Matrices , 1984 .

[61]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Diminishing Risk-Taking Behavior by Modulating Activity in the Prefrontal Cortex: A Direct Current Stimulation Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  R. Sutherland,et al.  The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[63]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  The Mental Cost of Cognitive Enhancement , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  M. Lövdén,et al.  No Significant Effect of Prefrontal tDCS on Working Memory Performance in Older Adults , 2015, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[65]  P. Turkeltaub,et al.  Differences in the experience of active and sham transcranial direct current stimulation , 2012, Brain Stimulation.