Pavlovian Blindsight and Masked Conditioning: A Neural Network Approach

Blindsight, the ability to respond to supraliminal visual stimuli despite extensive primary visual (V1) damage and reports of them as unseen, is central to theories of phenomenal consciousness. However, two related phenomena have not yet been considered in any theory: Pavlovian blindsight and masked conditioning. I propose a unified theoretical account of both by applying an existing neural network model of conditioning to the influential two-stream hypothesis of visual organization. On this hypothesis, V1 projects to the posterior parietal (dorsal) and inferotemporal cortices (ventral). The former also receives projections from the pulvinar, which allows for a V1-independent visual control, characteristic of blindsight. Artificial neural networks were designed after this hypothesis and according to the model. The model was originally proposed as a unified connectionist account of Pavlovian and operant conditioning, but the focus in this paper is on Pavlovian conditioning, where reinforcement (the occurrence of the unconditioned stimulus) is independent of responding. The model’s learning rule combines Hebbian learning with a form of reinforcement learning inspired by the roles of hippocampal and dopaminergic systems in both kinds of conditioning. This signal takes the form of a diffuse temporal difference in the activations of neurocomputational units that simulate hippocampal and dopaminergic systems. A network with pulvinar and without V1 inputs simulated Pavlovian blindsight. A network with both inputs and a larger ventral subnetwork simulated masked conditioning. Implications for the understanding of blindsight are discussed.

[1]  J. Donahoe,et al.  The S-R issue: its status in behavior analysis and in Donahoe and Palmer's learning and complex behavior. , 1997, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[2]  Gustavo Olague,et al.  Purposive Evolution for Object Recognition Using an Artificial Visual Cortex , 2012, EVOLVE.

[3]  Sigrid S. Glenn,et al.  A general account of selection: Biology, immunology, and behavior-Open Peer Commentary-A neural-network interpretation of selection in learning and behavior , 2001 .

[4]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[5]  S. Zeki,et al.  Toward a Theory of Visual Consciousness , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[6]  P. L. Brown,et al.  Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. , 1968, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[7]  Donald G. Marquis,et al.  CONDITIONED RESPONSES TO LIGHT IN MONKEYS AFTER REMOVAL OF THE OCCIPITAL LOBES , 1937 .

[8]  Volkmar Glauche,et al.  Ventral and dorsal pathways for language , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  J. Burgos Chapter 4 – Evolving Artificial Neural Networks in Pavlovian Environments , 1997 .

[10]  J W Donahoe,et al.  A selectionist approach to reinforcement. , 1993, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[11]  E. Schier,et al.  Identifying phenomenal consciousness , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[12]  M. Gallagher,et al.  The amygdala central nucleus and appetitive Pavlovian conditioning: lesions impair one class of conditioned behavior , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  J. Burgos Theoretical note: the C/T ratio in artificial neural networks , 2005, Behavioural Processes.

[15]  Christopher S. Hill Sensations: A Defense of Type Materialism , 1993 .

[16]  M. Tamietto,et al.  Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  D. Shanks,et al.  The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implications. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[18]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[19]  M. Dawson,et al.  Minds and Machines: Connectionism and Psychological Modeling , 2003 .

[20]  J. Burgos,et al.  Neural-network simulations of two context-dependence phenomena , 2007, Behavioural Processes.

[21]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  Juan M. Galeazzi,et al.  Some structural determinants of Pavlovian conditioning in artificial neural networks , 2010, Behavioural Processes.

[23]  M A Goodale,et al.  Visuomotor control: Where does vision end and action begin? , 1998, Current Biology.

[24]  J. Donahoe,et al.  Unified principle of reinforcement in a neural-network model: Reply to N. T. Calvin and J. J. McDowell , 2016, Behavioural Processes.

[25]  J. J. C. Smart,et al.  Sensations and brain processes , 1959 .

[26]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  U. T. Place,et al.  Is consciousness a brain process? , 1956, British journal of psychology.

[28]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: I. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  Harald T Schupp,et al.  Affective blindsight: intact fear conditioning to a visual cue in a cortically blind patient. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  D. Lloyd Consciousness, connectionism, and cognitive neuroscience: A meeting of the minds , 1996 .

[31]  J. Burgos,et al.  A simultaneous procedure facilitates acquisition under an optimal interstimulus interval in artificial neural networks and rats , 2008, Behavioural Processes.

[32]  Mohammad Saifullah A Biologically Inspired Model for Occluded Patterns , 2011, ICONIP.

[33]  M. Yukie,et al.  A direct projection from hippocampal field CA1 to ventral area TE of inferotemporal cortex in the monkey , 1988, Brain Research.

[34]  D. Chalmers The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory , 1996 .

[35]  V. Lamme Towards a true neural stance on consciousness , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Eris Chinellato,et al.  The neuroscience of vision-based grasping: a functional review for computational modeling and bio-inspired robotics. , 2009, Journal of integrative neuroscience.

[37]  J. Burgos Autoshaping and automaintenance: a neural-network approach. , 2007, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[38]  J. Donahoe,et al.  Behavior analysis and revaluation. , 2000, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[39]  Pamela Reinagel,et al.  Evidence That Primary Visual Cortex Is Required for Image, Orientation, and Motion Discrimination by Rats , 2013, PloS one.

[40]  L. Weiskrantz Blindsight : a case study and implications , 1986 .

[41]  P. H. Schiller On the specificity of neurons and visual areas , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[42]  Jonathan R. Whitlock,et al.  Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  W. Henton,et al.  Classical Conditioning and Operant Conditioning , 1978 .

[44]  Dan Lloyd,et al.  Consciousness: A connectionist manifesto , 1995, Minds and Machines.

[45]  M. Morgan,et al.  The Poggendorff illusion affects manual pointing as well as perceptual judgements , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  J. Burgos Theoretical note: simulating latent inhibition with selection neural networks , 2003, Behavioural Processes.

[47]  S. R. Coleman,et al.  Classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response at backward, simultaneous, and forward CS-US intervals. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  J. Donahoe,et al.  Pavlovian conditioning: Pigeon nictitating membrane , 2011, Behavioural Processes.

[49]  B. Baars A cognitive theory of consciousness , 1988 .

[50]  P. Mcmullen,et al.  Neuropsychological dissociations between motion and form perception suggest functional organization in extrastriate cortical regions in the human brain , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[52]  R. Dolan,et al.  A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating "unseen" fear. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[53]  J. Burgos Computational Models of Conditioning: The operant/respondent distinction: a computational neural-network analysis , 2010 .

[54]  Wulfram Gerstner,et al.  Theory and Simulation in Neuroscience , 2012, Science.

[55]  Gianluca Campana,et al.  Interactions between motion and form processing in the human visual system , 2013, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[56]  D. Dulany,et al.  Consciousness and agency: Explaining what and explaining who , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[57]  R. Rescorla Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.