Kinematics of Visually-Guided Eye Movements

One of the hallmarks of an eye movement that follows Listing’s law is the half-angle rule that says that the angular velocity of the eye tilts by half the angle of eccentricity of the line of sight relative to primary eye position. Since all visually-guided eye movements in the regime of far viewing follow Listing’s law (with the head still and upright), the question about its origin is of considerable importance. Here, we provide theoretical and experimental evidence that Listing’s law results from a unique motor strategy that allows minimizing ocular torsion while smoothly tracking objects of interest along any path in visual space. The strategy consists in compounding conventional ocular rotations in meridian planes, that is in horizontal, vertical and oblique directions (which are all torsion-free) with small linear displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. Such compound rotation-displacements of the eye can explain the kinematic paradox that the fixation point may rotate in one plane while the eye rotates in other planes. Its unique signature is the half-angle law in the position domain, which means that the rotation plane of the eye tilts by half-the angle of gaze eccentricity. We show that this law does not readily generalize to the velocity domain of visually-guided eye movements because the angular eye velocity is the sum of two terms, one associated with rotations in meridian planes and one associated with displacements of the eye in the frontal plane. While the first term does not depend on eye position the second term does depend on eye position. We show that compounded rotation - displacements perfectly predict the average smooth kinematics of the eye during steady- state pursuit in both the position and velocity domain.

[1]  Klaus Hepp,et al.  The Eye of a Mathematical Physicist , 2009 .

[2]  H. Helmholtz Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 2015 .

[3]  K Hepp,et al.  Role of Monkey Nucleus Reticularis Tegmenti Pontis in the Stabilization of Listing’s Plane , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  K Hepp,et al.  Monkey superior colliculus represents rapid eye movements in a two-dimensional motor map. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  D. Robinson Eye movements evoked by collicular stimulation in the alert monkey. , 1972, Vision research.

[6]  Anand C. Joshi,et al.  Three-dimensional kinematics of saccadic and pursuit eye movements in humans: Relationship between Donders’ and Listing’s laws , 2012, Vision Research.

[7]  B. Hess,et al.  Gravity modulates Listing's plane orientation during both pursuit and saccades. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Three-dimensional properties of human pursuit eye movements , 1992, Vision Research.

[9]  A. J. Van Opstal,et al.  Three-dimensional analysis of strongly curved saccades elicited by double-step stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  J. M. Miller,et al.  Innervation of extraocular pulley smooth muscle in monkeys and humans. , 1997, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[11]  J. Demer,et al.  Evidence for active control of rectus extraocular muscle pulleys. , 2000, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[12]  Ken Nakayama,et al.  Kinematics of normal and strabismic eyes , 1983 .

[13]  Gunnar Blohm,et al.  Visuomotor velocity transformations for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  K. Hepp,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements obey Listing's law in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Bernhard J M Hess,et al.  Quick phases control ocular torsion during smooth pursuit. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  R J Krauzlis,et al.  Shared motor error for multiple eye movements. , 1997, Science.

[17]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Three-dimensional ocular kinematics during eccentric rotations: evidence for functional rather than mechanical constraints. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Similar kinematic properties for ocular following and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  K. Hepp On Listing's law , 1990 .

[20]  R John Leigh,et al.  Three‐dimensional kinematics of saccadic eye movements in humans: is the “half‐angle rule” obeyed? , 2011, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Jean-Jacques Orban de Xivry,et al.  Saccades and pursuit: two outcomes of a single sensorimotor process , 2007, The Journal of physiology.

[22]  R. Hetherington The Perception of the Visual World , 1952 .

[23]  Peter S Sándor,et al.  Chameleon eye position obeys Listing's law , 2001, Vision Research.

[24]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Foveal Versus Full-Field Visual Stabilization Strategies for Translational and Rotational Head Movements , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  J. M. Miller,et al.  Evidence for fibromuscular pulleys of the recti extraocular muscles. , 1995, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[26]  K Hepp,et al.  Two- rather than three-dimensional representation of saccades in monkey superior colliculus. , 1991, Science.

[27]  T. Vilis,et al.  Geometric relations of eye position and velocity vectors during saccades , 1990, Vision Research.

[28]  P. Schiller,et al.  Discharge characteristics of single units in superior colliculus of the alert rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  B. J. M. Hess,et al.  Dual-search coil for measuring 3-dimensional eye movements in experimental animals , 1990, Vision Research.

[30]  Michael Fetter,et al.  Three-Dimensional Kinematics of Eye, Head and Limb Movements , 1997 .

[31]  D. Tweed,et al.  Three-dimensional model of the human eye-head saccadic system. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  K. Hepp,et al.  Calibration of three-dimensional eye position using search coil signals in the rhesus monkey , 1992, Vision Research.

[33]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. 3. Cells discharging before eye movements. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  David Hestenes,et al.  Invariant body kinematics: I. Saccadic and compensatory eye movements , 1994, Neural Networks.

[35]  F. Scheck Mechanics: From Newton's Laws to Deterministic Chaos , 2018 .

[36]  Jefferson E. Roy,et al.  Signal processing in the vestibular system during active versus passive head movements. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  D Tweed,et al.  Optimizing gaze control in three dimensions. , 1998, Science.

[38]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Neural Correlates of Forward and Inverse Models for Eye Movements: Evidence from Three-Dimensional Kinematics , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Werner Haustein,et al.  Considerations on Listing's Law and the primary position by means of a matrix description of eye position control , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[40]  R. Krauzlis The Control of Voluntary Eye Movements: New Perspectives , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[41]  T. Vilis,et al.  Computing three-dimensional eye position quaternions and eye velocity from search coil signals , 1990, Vision Research.

[42]  J. Demer,et al.  Active pulleys: magnetic resonance imaging of rectus muscle paths in tertiary gazes. , 2002, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[43]  Jefferson E. Roy,et al.  Brain stem pursuit pathways: dissociating visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive inputs during combined eye-head gaze tracking. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  B. Hess,et al.  Noncommutative control in the rotational vestibuloocular reflex. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  K. Hepp,et al.  Theoretical explanations of Listing's law and their implication for binocular vision , 1995, Vision Research.