Generality of the Laws of Learning: From Biological Constraints to Ecological Perspectives
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Domjan. The Garcia-Koelling Selective Association Effect: A Historical and Personal Perspective , 2015 .
[2] W. Timberlake. Integrating niche-related and general process approaches in the study of learning , 2001, Behavioural Processes.
[3] P. L. Brown,et al. Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. , 1968, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[4] David Barber,et al. Learning as inference , 2011 .
[5] R. Menzel,et al. Cognition in Invertebrates , 2007 .
[6] S. Shettleworth. Constraints on Learning , 1972 .
[7] A. Ǻgmo,et al. Sexual motivation—an inquiry into events determining the occurrence of sexual behavior , 1999, Behavioural Brain Research.
[8] J. Ayromlooi,et al. Oxytocin response to conditioned and nonconditioned stimuli in lactating ewes. , 1987, Biology of reproduction.
[9] John Garcia,et al. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .
[10] P. Rozin,et al. Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. , 1971, Psychological review.
[11] M. Domjan,et al. Dissociation of conditioned appetitive and consummatory sexual behavior: Satiation and extinction tests , 1998 .
[12] R. Rescorla,et al. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .
[13] M. Domjan,et al. Contribution of ingestive behaviors to taste-aversion learning in the rat. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[14] Robert Ian Bowers,et al. An animal model of fetishism. , 2004, Behaviour research and therapy.
[15] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[16] W. Timberlake,et al. Behavior Systems and Learning: From Misbehavior to General Principles , 2019, Contemporary Learning Theories.
[17] K. Hollis,et al. The biological function of Pavlovian conditioning: A mechanism for mating success in the blue gourami (Trichogaster trichopterus). , 1989 .
[18] R. Mcdonald,et al. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Pavlovian Psychopharmacology: the Associative Basis of Tolerance , 2022 .
[19] K. Hollis. The role of Pavlovian conditioning in territorial aggression and reproduction. , 1990 .
[20] L. Parker,et al. Aversions to unflavored water and cup drinking produced by delayed sickness. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[21] Emily G. Gean. Testing the validity of a non-associative theory of Pavlovian learning, rate estimation theory (RET) , 2006 .
[22] C. B. Ferster,et al. Schedules of reinforcement , 1957 .
[23] A. Logue. Taste aversion and the generality of the laws of learning. , 1979 .
[24] E. Adkins-Regan,et al. Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) inseminations are more likely to fertilize eggs in a context predicting mating opportunities , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[25] William Timberlake,et al. Motivational modes in behavior systems , 2000 .
[26] J. Pearce,et al. A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980 .
[27] S. J. Weiss,et al. Hedonics and the "Selective Associations": Biological Constraint on Learning , 2015 .
[28] W. Timberlake. Natural learning in laboratory paradigms. , 1990 .
[29] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Time as content in Pavlovian conditioning , 1998, Behavioural Processes.
[30] R. Rescorla. Associative Changes with a Random CS-US Relationship , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.
[31] Barry Schwartz,et al. On going back to nature: a review of Seligman and Hager's Biological boundaries of learning , 1974 .
[32] Classical conditioning increases reproductive success in Japanese quail, Coturnix japonica , 2005, Animal Behaviour.
[33] James C. Smith,et al. Trace conditioning with X-rays as an aversive stimulus , 1967 .
[34] R. Bolles. Species-Specific Defense Reactions , 1971 .
[35] Operant conditioning of scratching in lemurs , 1990, Primates.
[36] G. Davey,et al. Preparedness and phobias: Specific evolved associations or a generalized expectancy bias? , 1995, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[37] M. Domjan,et al. Extinction of conditioned sexual responses in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica): role of species-typical cues. , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[38] A. R. Wagner. SOP: A Model of Automatic Memory Processing in Animal Behavior , 2014 .
[39] M. Domjan,et al. Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: Retrospect and prospect , 1983 .
[40] N. Mackintosh. A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .
[41] S. Siegel. The Heroin Overdose Mystery , 2016 .
[42] Poison-avoidance learning to food-related tactile stimuli: Avoidance of texture cues by rats , 1982 .
[43] A. Dickinson. Contemporary Animal Learning Theory , 1981 .
[44] Ralph R. Miller,et al. Sometimes-competing retrieval (SOCR): a formalization of the comparator hypothesis. , 2007, Psychological review.
[45] S. Siegel. Drug Tolerance, Drug Addiction, and Drug Anticipation , 2005 .
[46] J. L. Gould,et al. Is tonic immobility adaptive? , 1984, Animal Behaviour.
[47] V. Lolordo,et al. Attention in the pigeon: differential effects of food-getting versus shock-avoidance procedures. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[48] S. Siegel,et al. DRUG-ONSET CUES AS SIGNALS : INTRAADMINISTRATION ASSOCIATIONS AND TOLERANCE , 1999 .
[49] Michael Domjan,et al. Ingestional Aversion Learning: Unique and General Processes , 1980 .
[50] A. Ågmo. Unconditioned sexual incentive motivation in the male Norway rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[51] J. Rzóska. Bait shyness, a study in rat behaviour , 1953 .
[52] R. Bolles. Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. , 1970 .
[53] Signalled and unsignalled free-operant avoidance in the pigeon. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[54] W. Timberlake,et al. Stimulus and response contingencies in the misbehavior of rats. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[55] C. W. Schwartz,et al. A reconnaissance of the game birds in Hawaii , 1949 .
[56] J. Brener,et al. Avoidance conditioning of activity and immobility in rats. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[57] G. Britton,et al. Classical conditioning provides paternity advantage for territorial male blue gouramis (Trichogaster trichopterus). , 1997 .
[58] D. R. Williams,et al. Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[59] M. Domjan. Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: implications for general process theory , 1983 .
[60] M. Domjan,et al. Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat: Control for US-induced differential orientations , 1981 .
[61] Note on aversion learning to the shape of food by monkeys. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[62] J. Staddon. Adaptive behavior and learning , 1983 .
[63] J. Maatsch. Learning and fixation after a single shock trial. , 1959, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[64] D. Schams,et al. The effects of conditioning to suckling, milking and of calf presence on the release of oxytocin in dairy cows. , 2001, Applied animal behaviour science.
[65] M. Krause. Evolutionary perspectives on learning: conceptual and methodological issues in the study of adaptive specializations , 2015, Animal Cognition.
[66] John Garcia,et al. Behavioral Regulation of the Milieu Interne in Man and Rat , 1974, Science.
[67] William Timberlake,et al. Feeding ecology and laboratory predatory behavior toward live and artificial moving prey in seven rodent species , 1989 .
[68] R. Menzel,et al. Average group behavior does not represent individual behavior in classical conditioning of the honeybee. , 2011, Learning & memory.
[69] R. M. Elliott,et al. Behavior of Organisms , 1991 .
[70] B. Schwartz. Maintenance of keypecking in pigeons by a food avoidance but not by a shock avoidance contingency , 1973 .
[71] E. G. Johnson,et al. Previous sexual experience alters the display of paced mating behavior in female rats , 2014, Hormones and Behavior.
[72] T. Davidson,et al. A role for sweet taste: calorie predictive relations in energy regulation by rats. , 2008, Behavioral neuroscience.
[73] D. Stephens,et al. Experimental evolution of prepared learning , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[74] T. Davidson,et al. A view of obesity as a learning and memory disorder. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[75] M. Domjan,et al. Special Efficacy of Sexual Conditioned Stimuli That Include Species Typical Cues: Tests with a Conditioned Stimuli Preexposure Design , 1998 .
[76] R. Boakes,et al. A study of misbehavior: token reinforcement in the rat. , 1978, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[77] M. Domjan,et al. Blocking of the sexual conditioning of differentially effective conditioned stimulus objects , 1994 .
[78] M. Domjan,et al. Learning in intimate connections: Conditioned fertility and its role in sexual competition , 2012, Socioaffective neuroscience & psychology.
[79] S. Shettleworth. Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: Hunger, environment, and food reinforcement. , 1975 .
[80] E. Thorndike. Animal Intelligence; Experimental Studies , 2009 .
[81] S. Siegel,et al. Applying laboratory research: drug anticipation and the treatment of drug addiction. , 2002, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.
[82] M. Fanselow,et al. Neurobehavioral perspectives on the distinction between fear and anxiety , 2015, Learning & memory.
[83] Jose L. Pardo-Vazquez,et al. Differential effects of forward or simultaneous conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus intervals on the defensive behavior system of the Norway rat (Rattus norvegicus). , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[84] D. H. Owings,et al. Effects of predator training on behavior and post-release survival of captive prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus) , 2006 .
[85] J. Woodson. Including ‘learned sexuality’ in the organization of sexual behavior , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[86] J. Ayres,et al. Factors affecting conditioning in the truly random control procedure in the rat. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[87] K. Breland,et al. The misbehavior of organisms. , 1961 .
[88] S. Mineka,et al. Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.
[89] G. Gallup,et al. An ethological analysis of open-field behavior in rats and mice , 1981 .
[90] A. Alonso,et al. Sexual behavior regulated (paced) by the female induces conditioned place preference. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.
[91] C. Gallistel,et al. Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.
[92] J. Pfaus,et al. The role of oxytocin and vasopressin in conditioned mate guarding behavior in the female rat , 2015, Physiology & Behavior.
[93] Douglas P. Chivers,et al. Learning threat-sensitive predator avoidance: how do fathead minnows incorporate conflicting information? , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[94] E. Blass. Suckling : determinants, changes, mechanisms, and lasting impressions , 1990 .
[95] M. Domjan,et al. Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat , 1972 .
[96] S. Shettleworth,et al. Behavioral adaptation to fixed-interval and fixed-time food delivery in golden hamsters. , 1977, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[97] A. R. Wagner,et al. Evolution of a structured connectionist model of Pavlovian conditioning (AESOP). , 1989 .
[98] I. Bernstein,et al. Biologically predisposed learning and selective associations in amygdalar neurons. , 2011, Learning & memory.
[99] M. Domjan. Role of ingestion in odor-toxicosis learning in the rat. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[100] H. M. Jenkins,et al. Sign-tracking : the stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action , 1974 .
[101] S. J. Weiss,et al. Sensory modality and stimulus control in the pigeon: Cross-species generality of single-incentive selective-association effects , 2005 .
[102] W. Young,et al. Postweaning, forebrain‐specific perturbation of the oxytocin system impairs fear conditioning , 2011, Genes, brain, and behavior.
[103] J. Pearce,et al. Instrumental conditioning of scratching in the laboratory rat , 1978 .
[104] Daniel T. Blumstein,et al. Learning specificity in acquired predator recognition , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[105] Steve Reilly,et al. Conditioned taste aversion : behavioral and neural processes , 2009 .
[106] M. Morgan,et al. Discrimination between reinforced action patterns in the rat , 1979 .
[107] Michael S. Fanselow,et al. A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. , 1988 .
[108] M. R. D AMATO,et al. LONG-TERM DISCRIMINATED AVOIDANCE PERFORMANCE IN THE RAT. , 1964, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[109] R. Menzel. The honeybee as a model for understanding the basis of cognition , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[110] V. Lolordo,et al. The sensory basis of avoidance responding in the rat: Relative dominance of auditory or visual warning signals and safety signals , 1977 .
[111] S. Centeno,et al. Conditioning and Sexual Behavior: A Review , 2001, Hormones and Behavior.
[112] M. Domjan,et al. Learning with arbitrary versus ecological conditioned stimuli: Evidence from sexual conditioning , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[113] M. Krause. Behavioral mechanisms and the neurobiology of conditioned sexual responding. , 2003, International review of neurobiology.
[114] Pavlovian conditioning of sexual arousal: first- and second-order effects. , 1985 .
[115] E. Hilgard,et al. Theories of Learning , 1981 .
[116] C. Akins. Effects of Species-Specific Cues and the CS–US Interval on the Topography of the Sexually Conditioned Response , 2000 .
[117] P. Rozin,et al. Role of interference in taste-aversion learning. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[118] M. Domjan. Pavlovian conditioning: a functional perspective. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.
[119] M. Domjan,et al. Selective associations in one-day-old rats: taste-toxicosis and texture-shock aversion learning. , 1982, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[120] M. Domjan,et al. Applications of Pavlovian Conditioning to Sexual Behavior and Reproduction , 2011 .
[121] John Garcia,et al. Learning with prolonged delay of reinforcement , 1966 .
[122] V. Miller. Selective association learning in the rat: Generality of response system ☆ , 1984 .
[123] M. Seligman. On the generality of the laws of learning , 1970 .