Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules

Many of the learning difficulties in the specific domain of chemistry are found not only in the ideas already possessed by students but in the strategic and procedural knowledge that is characteristic of everyday thinking. These defects in procedural knowledge have been described as functional fixedness and functional reduction. This article assesses the procedural difficulties of students (grade 12 and first and third year of university) based on common sense reasoning in two areas of chemistry: chemical equilibrium and geometry and polarity of molecules. In the first area, the theme of external factors affecting equilibria (temperature and concentration change) was selected because the explanations given by the students could be analyzed easily. The existence of a functional fixedness where Le Chatelier's principle was almost exclusively applied by rote could be observed, with this being the cause of the incorrect responses given to the proposed items. Functional fixedness of the Lewis structure also led to an incorrect prediction of molecular geometry. When molecular geometry was correctly determined by the students, it seemed that other methodological or procedural difficulties appeared when the task was to determine molecular polarity. The students showed a tendency, in many cases, to reduce the factors affecting molecular polarity in two possible ways: (a) assuming that polarity depends only on shape (“geometric functional reduction”) or (b) assuming that molecular polarity depends only on the polarity of bonds (“bonding functional reduction”). © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Sci Ed84:545–565, 2000.

[1]  G. Crosby Structure, bonding, and excited states of coordination complexes , 1983 .

[2]  J. Minstrell Explaining the ’’at rest’’ condition of an object , 1982 .

[3]  Roger Osborne,et al.  Learning in science , 1985 .

[4]  P. Hewson,et al.  The conditions of conceptual change in the classroom , 1989 .

[5]  D. Kuhn Science as argument : Implications for teaching and learning scientific thinking , 1993 .

[6]  Ibrahim A. Halloun,et al.  Common sense concepts about motion , 1985 .

[7]  H. Birch,et al.  The negative effect of previous experience on productive thinking. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  P. Hewson,et al.  Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change , 1982 .

[9]  M. Hewson,et al.  The Influence of Intellectual Environment on Conceptions of Heat. , 1984 .

[10]  Maher Z. Hashweh Toward an explanation of conceptual change , 1986 .

[11]  David F. Treagust,et al.  Development and Application of a Diagnostic Instrument to Evaluate Grade-11 and -12 Students' Concepts of Covalent Bonding and Structure Following a Course of Instruction. , 1989 .

[12]  John R. Anderson Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications , 1980 .

[13]  Norman Frederiksen,et al.  Implications of Cognitive Theory for Instruction in Problem Solving , 1984 .

[14]  Ron Good,et al.  Problem solving and chemical equilibrium: Successful versus unsuccessful performance , 1989 .

[15]  B. Andersson,et al.  The experiential gestalt of causation: a common core to pupils’ preconceptions in science , 1986 .

[16]  D. Gil-Pérez,et al.  Bringing pupils' learning closer to a scientific construction of knowledge: A permanent feature in innovations in science teaching , 1994 .

[17]  Henry Heikkinen,et al.  Student ideas regarding chemical equilibrium: What written test answers do not reveal , 1990 .

[18]  R. Driver,et al.  Students’ conceptions and the learning of science , 1989 .

[19]  Richard T. White,et al.  Metalearning and conceptual change , 1989 .

[20]  Peter W. Hewson,et al.  Epistemological commitments in the learning of science: Examples from dynamics , 1985 .

[21]  Peter W. Hewson,et al.  Effect of instruction using microcomputer simulations and conceptual change strategies on science learning , 1986 .

[22]  J. Nussbaum,et al.  Alternative frameworks, conceptual conflict and accommodation: Toward a principled teaching strategy , 1982 .

[23]  Jean-Louis Closset,et al.  Raisonnements en électricité et en hydrodynamique , 1992 .

[24]  R. Duschl,et al.  Epistemological perspectives on conceptual change: Implications for educational practice , 1991 .

[25]  Thomas J. Shuell,et al.  Cognitive psychology and conceptual change: Implications for teaching science† , 1987 .

[26]  R. Gunstone,et al.  Developments in style and purpose of research on the learning of science , 1988 .

[27]  L. Viennot RAISONNEMENT A PLUSIEURS VARIABLES TENDANCES DE LA PENSÉE COMMUNE , 1992 .

[28]  C. Furió,et al.  Difficulties with the Geometry and Polarity of Molecules: Beyond Misconceptions , 1996 .

[29]  Reinders Duit,et al.  Students' Alternative Frameworks and Science Education. Bibliography. 3rd Edition. IPN Reports-in-Brief = Alltagsvorstellungen und Naturwissenschaftlicher Unterricht. Bibliographie. 3. Auflage. IPN-Kurzberichte. , 1991 .

[30]  Robert J. Whitaker,et al.  Aristotle is not dead: Student understanding of trajectory motion , 1983 .

[31]  Daniel Perez,et al.  Science learning as a conceptual and methodological change , 1985 .

[32]  P. Cobb,et al.  Analogies from the philosophy and sociology of science for understanding classroom life , 1991 .