Mental representation of prepositional compounds: Evidence from Italian agrammatic patients

The processing of Prepositional compounds (typical Neo-latin noun--noun modifications where a head noun is modified by a prepositional phrase, e.g., mulino a vento, windmill) was preliminarily studied with a group of six agrammatic aphasic patients, and, in more detail, with a further agrammatic patient (MB). Omission was the most frequent error type in naming, whereas in the other tasks (repetition, reading, writing, and completion) errors were mostly substitutions of the target preposition. This happened even with fully lexicalized compound forms, i.e., those forms where the linking preposition is syntactically and semantically opaque. These findings are interpreted in terms of a dual-route theory of lexical access to morphologically complex words.

[1]  S. Blumstein,et al.  Frequency, picturability and availability of nouns in aphasia. , 1969, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[2]  R. Baayen,et al.  Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model , 1997 .

[3]  Caterina Pistarini,et al.  The mental representation of Verb–Noun compounds in Italian: Evidence from a multiple single-case study in aphasia , 2004, Brain and Language.

[4]  Gary Libben,et al.  The Representation and Processing of Compound Words , 2006 .

[5]  Caterina Pistarini,et al.  Verb–Noun Double Dissociation in Aphasic Lexical Impairments: The Role of Word Frequency and Imageability , 2002, Brain and Language.

[6]  Thomas Benke,et al.  Naming by German compounds , 1994, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[7]  F Michel,et al.  Deep dysphasia: An analog of deep dyslexia in the auditory modality , 1983, Brain and Language.

[8]  R. C. Oldfield,et al.  The Time it Takes to Name an Object , 1964, Nature.

[9]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Disorders of phonological encoding , 1992, Cognition.

[10]  R. D. Bock,et al.  Psycholinguistic study of aphasia: a revision of the concept of anomia. , 1956, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[11]  Cristina Burani,et al.  Why Is “Red Cross” Different from “Yellow Cross”?: A Neuropsychological Study of Noun–Adjective Agreement within Italian Compounds , 2002, Brain and Language.

[12]  Anne Cutler,et al.  A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[14]  Gerhard Blanken,et al.  The Production of Nominal Compounds in Aphasia , 2000, Brain and Language.

[15]  Brenda C Rapp,et al.  Handbook of Cognitive Neuropsychology: What Deficits Reveal About the Human Mind , 2001 .

[16]  C. Westbury,et al.  Semantics and semantic errors: Implicit access to semantic information from words and nonwords in deep dyslexia , 2003, Brain and Language.

[17]  Margarete Delazer,et al.  The Processing of Compound Words: A Study in Aphasia , 1998, Brain and Language.

[18]  W. Badecker,et al.  Morphology: The internal structure of words , 2001 .

[19]  W. Levelt,et al.  Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form , 1994 .

[20]  Ria De Bleser,et al.  Morphological Processing in Italian Agrammatic Speakers: Eight Experiments in Lexical Morphology , 1996, Brain and Language.

[21]  C. Semenza,et al.  Lexical Representation and Processing of Morphologically Complex Words: Evidence from the Reading Performance of an Italian Agrammatic Patient , 2001, Brain and Language.

[22]  G. Dell,et al.  Stages of lexical access in language production , 1992, Cognition.

[23]  C. Semenza,et al.  Morphological representation of compound nouns: A study on Italian aphasic patients , 1997, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[24]  M. Garrett Disorders of lexical selection , 1992, Cognition.

[25]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .