Interspecific acoustic interactions among katydids Neoconocephalus: inhibition-induced shifts in diel periodicity

Abstract Males of the katydid Neoconocephalus spiza stridulate in a loud, chirping manner, but they are inhibited from singing during the uninterrupted songs of three sympatric congeners. Playback experiments using natural and synthetic stimuli showed that inhibition can be caused by any continuous or rapidly pulsing sound presented at an intensity greater than 40 dB sound pressure level and from 8 to 16 kHz in frequency, the approximate spectral range of the N. spiza chirps and the continuous songs of the congeneric species. In specific habitats where inhibiting species are absent, N. spiza sings primarily at night, but diurnal singing prevails where the inhibitors, all nocturnal, are found. Removal of the inhibiting species in a field experiment resulted in a rapid shift of the periodicity of N. spiza signalling from diurnal to nocturnal. This is the first reported case of a reversal in diel periodicity of singing caused by signal interference. It is argued that in a noisy environment, long-term temporal adjustments may be the most effective means of avoiding the problem of acoustic interference.

[1]  J. Rheinlaender,et al.  Temporal parameters of male—female sound communication in Leptophyes punctatissima , 1986 .

[2]  T. J. Walker,et al.  Singing Schedules and Sites for a Tropical Burrowing Cricket (Anurogryllus muticus) , 1982 .

[3]  Acoustical behavior and spacing in the Nebraska Conehead Neoconocephalus nebrascensis (Bruner)- (Orthoptera: Tettingoniidae) , 1976 .

[4]  K. C. Shaw An Analysis of the Phonoresponse of Males of the True Katydid, Pterophylla Camellifolia (Fabricius) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) , 1968 .

[5]  Unsynchronized chorusing in the coneheaded katydid Neoconocephalus affinis (Beauvois) , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  S. Telford,et al.  Influence of the acoustic environment on the distribution of the frog Ranidella riparia , 1986, Animal Behaviour.

[7]  James H. Brown,et al.  Song Asynchrony in Neighbouring Bird Species , 1969, Nature.

[8]  Michael D Greenfield,et al.  Phonotaxis and aggression in the coneheaded katydid Neoconocephalus affinis , 1985 .

[9]  M. Littlejohn,et al.  Acoustic interaction between two species of leptodactylid frogs , 1969 .

[10]  K. Wells,et al.  Intra- and Interspecific Vocal Behavior of the Neotropical Treefrog Hyla microcephala , 1985 .

[11]  M. Samways Habitats and habits of Platycleis spp. (Orthoptera, Tettigoniidae) in southern France , 1976 .

[12]  B. Sullivan Sexual Selection in the Great Plains Toad (Bufo Cognatus) , 1983 .

[13]  Song modification in the Orthoptera II. Types of acoustic interaction between Platycleis intermedia and other species of the genus (Tettigoniidae) , 1976 .

[14]  T. J. Walker,et al.  Acoustic Synchrony: Two Mechanisms in the Snowy Tree Cricket , 1969, Science.

[15]  Acoustic competition in bush crickets , 1981 .

[16]  K. Wells,et al.  An experimental study of acoustic interference between two species of neotropical treefrogs , 1983, Animal Behaviour.

[17]  W. Latimer,et al.  Acoustic interference in bush crickets; a factor in the evolution of singing insects? , 1984 .

[18]  K. Wells,et al.  THE INFLUENCE OF BACKGROUND NOISE ON THE BEHAVIOR OF A NEOTROPICAL TREEFROG, HYLA EBRACCATA , 1983 .

[19]  J. Hailman,et al.  Temporal Pattern Shifts to Avoid Acoustic Interference in Singing Birds , 1974, Science.

[20]  R. W. Ficken,et al.  Short-term temporal avoidance of interspecific acoustic interference among forest birds , 1985 .