Mouth gape angle has little effect on the transmitted signals of big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus).

Bats perform high-resolution echolocation by comparing temporal and spectral features of their transmitted pulses to the received echoes. In complex environments with moving prey, dynamically adapting the transmitted pulses can increase the probability of successful target representation and interception. This study further investigates the adaptive vocal-motor strategies of big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). During stationary target detection experiments, echolocation sounds were simultaneously recorded with high-speed, infrared video to examine the relationship of mouth position and movement to pulse characteristics among bats. All three bats produced strobe groups, but the proportion and frequency characteristics of the strobe group pulses differed for individual bats. Additionally, mouth gape angle had little effect on the emitted pulse characteristics, which suggests that laryngeal mechanisms drive changes in emitted pulses.

[1]  J. Simmons The resolution of target range by echolocating bats. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[2]  C. Moss,et al.  Active Listening for Spatial Orientation in a Complex Auditory Scene , 2006, PLoS biology.

[3]  N Suga,et al.  Echo-Ranging Neurons in the Inferior Colliculus of Bats , 1970, Science.

[4]  C F Moss,et al.  Auditory scene analysis by echolocation in bats. , 2001, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[5]  R. Lindsay,et al.  Listening in the Dark , 1958 .

[6]  Louis W. Gellermann Chance Orders of Alternating Stimuli in Visual Discrimination Experiments , 1933 .

[7]  D. Hartley,et al.  The sound emission pattern of the echolocating bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 1989 .

[8]  N Suga,et al.  Target range-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. , 1979, Science.

[9]  C. Moss,et al.  The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[10]  J. Simmons,et al.  Echolocation and pursuit of prey by bats. , 1979, Science.

[11]  James A. Simmons,et al.  The structure of echolocation sounds used by the big brown bat Eptesicus fuscus : some consequences for echo processing , 1991 .

[12]  Hiroshi Riquimaroux,et al.  FM echolocating bats shift frequencies to avoid broadcast–echo ambiguity in clutter , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  James A Simmons,et al.  Multi-component separation and analysis of bat echolocation calls. , 2013, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  R. Suthers,et al.  Mechanisms of Sound Production by Echolocating Bats , 1973 .

[15]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Spatial perception and adaptive sonar behavior. , 2010, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[16]  James A Simmons,et al.  Bats use a neuronally implemented computational acoustic model to form sonar images , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  J. Simmons,et al.  Spectral cues and perception of the vertical position of targets by the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[18]  Lasse Jakobsen,et al.  Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  Hiroshi Riquimaroux,et al.  On-board telemetry of emitted sounds from free-flying bats: compensation for velocity and distance stabilizes echo frequency and amplitude , 2008, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.