Superfluous Neuroscience Information Makes Explanations of Psychological Phenomena More Appealing

Does the presence of irrelevant neuroscience information make explanations of psychological phenomena more appealing? Do fMRI pictures further increase that allure? To help answer these questions, 385 college students in four experiments read brief descriptions of psychological phenomena, each one accompanied by an explanation of varying quality (good vs. circular) and followed by superfluous information of various types. Ancillary measures assessed participants' analytical thinking, beliefs on dualism and free will, and admiration for different sciences. In Experiment 1, superfluous neuroscience information increased the judged quality of the argument for both good and bad explanations, whereas accompanying fMRI pictures had no impact above and beyond the neuroscience text, suggesting a bias that is conceptual rather than pictorial. Superfluous neuroscience information was more alluring than social science information (Experiment 2) and more alluring than information from prestigious “hard sciences” (Experiments 3 and 4). Analytical thinking did not protect against the neuroscience bias, nor did a belief in dualism or free will. We conclude that the “allure of neuroscience” bias is conceptual, specific to neuroscience, and not easily accounted for by the prestige of the discipline. It may stem from the lay belief that the brain is the best explanans for mental phenomena.

[1]  N. Schwarz,et al.  Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Judgments of Truth , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Implicit Philosophies of Mind: The Dualism Scale and Its Relation to Religiosity and Belief in Extrasensory Perception , 1989 .

[3]  Jennelle E. Yopchick,et al.  The influence of causal information on judgments of treatment efficacy , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[4]  Evan F. Risko,et al.  Fooled by the brain: Re-examining the influence of neuroimages , 2013, Cognition.

[5]  Deena Skolnick Weisberg,et al.  The Seductive Allure of Neuroscience Explanations , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  D. Paulhus,et al.  The FAD–Plus: Measuring Lay Beliefs Regarding Free Will and Related Constructs , 2011, Journal of personality assessment.

[7]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Look Again: Effects of Brain Images and Mind–Brain Dualism on Lay Evaluations of Research , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  H. Markovits,et al.  The belief-bias effect in the production and evaluation of logical conclusions , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[9]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[10]  J. Keller In genes we trust: the biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  David R. Gruber,et al.  Persuasive images in popular science: Testing judgments of scientific reasoning and credibility , 2012, Public understanding of science.

[12]  G. Bates History of genetic disease: The molecular genetics of Huntington disease — a history , 2005, Nature Reviews Genetics.

[13]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Instructional Manipulation Checks: Detecting Satisficing to Increase Statistical Power , 2009 .

[14]  Geoff Cumming,et al.  On the (non)persuasive power of a brain image , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Kimmo Eriksson The nonsense math effect , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[16]  Woo-kyoung Ahn,et al.  Mental Health Clinicians' Beliefs About the Biological, Psychological, and Environmental Bases of Mental Disorders , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[17]  Alan D. Castel,et al.  Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning , 2008, Cognition.

[18]  F. Keil,et al.  A bump on a bump? Emerging intuitions concerning the relative difficulty of the sciences. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al.  Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  P. S. Vivekananthan,et al.  A multidimensional approach to the structure of personality impressions. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Edward B. Royzman,et al.  Explaining Away Responsibility: Effects of Scientific Explanation on Perceived Culpability , 2005 .

[22]  Joshua Knobe,et al.  Dual character concepts and the normative dimension of conceptual representation , 2013, Cognition.

[23]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Different clues from different views: The role of image format in public perceptions of neuroimaging results , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  M. Masson Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[25]  P. Lunt,et al.  The perceived causal structure of loneliness. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.