Evaluating self-generated decisions in frontal pole cortex of monkeys

The frontal pole cortex (FPC) expanded markedly during human evolution, but its function remains uncertain in both monkeys and humans. Accordingly, we examined single-cell activity in this area. On every trial, monkeys decided between two response targets on the basis of a 'stay' or 'shift' cue. Feedback followed at a fixed delay. FPC cells did not encode the monkeys' decisions when they were made, but did so later on, as feedback approached. This finding indicates that the FPC is involved in monitoring or evaluating decisions. Using a control task and delayed feedback, we found that decision coding lasted until feedback only when the monkeys combined working memory with sensory cues to 'self-generate' decisions, as opposed to when they simply followed trial-by-trial instructions. A role in monitoring or evaluating self-generated decisions could account for FPC's expansion during human evolution.

[1]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  J B Poline,et al.  The neural system that bridges reward and cognition in humans: An fMRI study , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  R. E. Passingham,et al.  Prediction error for free monetary reward in the human prefrontal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.

[4]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[5]  Hatim A. Zariwala,et al.  Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence , 2008, Nature.

[6]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  How Green Is the Grass on the Other Side? Frontopolar Cortex and the Evidence in Favor of Alternative Courses of Action , 2009, Neuron.

[7]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  K. Sakai Task set and prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[11]  F. Schmidt Meta-Analysis , 2008 .

[12]  Andrew R. Mitz,et al.  A method for recording single-cell activity in the frontal-pole cortex of macaque monkeys , 2009, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[13]  A. Walker,et al.  A cytoarchitectural study of the prefrontal area of the macaque monkey , 1940 .

[14]  John D E Gabrieli,et al.  Evaluating self-generated information: anterior prefrontal contributions to human cognition. , 2003, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Response to Comment on "Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought" , 2007, Science.

[16]  T. Powell,et al.  An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. , 1970, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  S. Bunge How we use rules to select actions: A review of evidence from cognitive neuroscience , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[18]  Katsuyuki Sakai,et al.  Prefrontal Set Activity Predicts Rule-Specific Neural Processing during Subsequent Cognitive Performance , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Evelyn C. Ferstl,et al.  The Anterior Frontomedian Cortex and Evaluative Judgment: An fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  J. Price,et al.  Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex , 2003, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[21]  D. Pandya,et al.  Efferent Association Pathways from the Rostral Prefrontal Cortex in the Macaque Monkey , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  S. Mithen The Prehistory of the Mind , 1996 .

[23]  Fernando Zelaya,et al.  Distinct roles of prefrontal cortical subregions in the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[25]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[27]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Transient neuronal correlations underlying goal selection and maintenance in prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[29]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes , 2002, Neuron.

[30]  J. Jacobs,et al.  Regional dendritic and spine variation in human cerebral cortex: a quantitative golgi study. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  M. Phillips,et al.  Interacting outcome retrieval, anticipation, and feedback processes in the human brain. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Time Discounting for Primary Rewards , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Aldo Genovesio,et al.  Representation of Future and Previous Spatial Goals by Separate Neural Populations in Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  C. Frith,et al.  Differential involvement of regions of rostral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann area 10) in time- and event-based prospective memory. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[35]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  O. Sporns,et al.  Mapping the Structural Core of Human Cerebral Cortex , 2008, PLoS biology.

[37]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. , 2001, American journal of physical anthropology.

[38]  Justin M Ream,et al.  Neural Basis of Spontaneous thought Processes , 2004, Cortex.

[39]  A. Mitz A liquid-delivery device that provides precise reward control for neurophysiological and behavioral experiments , 2005, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[40]  M. Brass,et al.  Internally generated and directly cued task sets: an investigation with fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  A. V. van Duijvenvoorde,et al.  Evaluating the Negative or Valuing the Positive? Neural Mechanisms Supporting Feedback-Based Learning across Development , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  R. Veit,et al.  The truth about lying: inhibition of the anterior prefrontal cortex improves deceptive behavior. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  S. Wise,et al.  Encoding problem‐solving strategies in prefrontal cortex: activity during strategic errors , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  The Representation of Abstract Task Rules in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  Andrew R. Mitz,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Activity Related to Abstract Response Strategies , 2005, Neuron.

[48]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural correlates of different types of deception: an fMRI investigation. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[50]  Steven P. Wise,et al.  Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function , 2008, Trends in Neurosciences.