Weighted Integration Suggests That Visual and Tactile Signals Provide Independent Estimates About Duration

Humans might possess either a single (amodal) internal clock or multiple clocks for different sensory modalities. Sensitivity could be improved by the provision of multiple signals. Such improvements can be predicted quantitatively, assuming estimates are combined by summation, a process described as optimal when summation is weighted in accordance with the variance associated with each of the initially independent estimates. This possibility was assessed for visual and tactile information regarding temporal intervals. In Experiment 1, 12 musicians and 12 nonmusicians judged durations of 300 and 600 ms, compared to test values spanning these standards. Bimodal precision increased relative to unimodal conditions, but not to the extent predicted by optimally weighted summation. In Experiment 2, 6 musicians and 6 other participants each judged 6 standards, ranging from 100 ms to 600 ms, with conflicting cues providing a measure of the weight assigned to each sensory modality. A weighted integration model best fitted these data, with musicians more likely to select near-optimal weights than nonmusicians. Overall, data were consistent with the existence of separate visual and tactile clock components at either the counter/integrator or memory stages. Independent estimates are passed to a decisional process, but not always combined in a statistically optimal fashion.

[1]  G. Sandini,et al.  Development of Visuo-Auditory Integration in Space and Time , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[2]  R. Miall,et al.  Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[3]  A. Vendrik,et al.  CAN A SENSORY SYSTEM BE SPECIFIED BY ITS INTERNAL NOISE? , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[4]  A. Nobre,et al.  Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectation with fMRI , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  John E. Schlerf,et al.  Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[6]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Temporal mechanisms of multimodal binding , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  Ellen Poliakoff,et al.  Good vibrations: Human interval timing in the vibrotactile modality , 2009, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  A. Johnston,et al.  Duration Judgments Over Multiple Elements , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[9]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[10]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[11]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  Optimal integration of auditory and vibrotactile information for judgments of temporal order. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  Pavlos C Filippopoulos,et al.  Interference between auditory and visual duration judgements suggests a common code for time , 2013, Psychological research.

[13]  R. J. van Beers,et al.  Integration of proprioceptive and visual position-information: An experimentally supported model. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J. Wearden,et al.  Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations , 2006 .

[15]  D. Burr,et al.  Optimal Encoding of Interval Timing in Expert Percussionists , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  C. Chubb,et al.  A 'dipper' function for texture discrimination based on orientation variance. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[17]  M. Morgan,et al.  A single "stopwatch" for duration estimation, a single "ruler" for size. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[18]  J. Staddon,et al.  Interval timing , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  David Whitaker,et al.  Duration channels mediate human time perception , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Zhuanghua Shi,et al.  Reducing Bias in Auditory Duration Reproduction by Integrating the Reproduced Signal , 2013, PloS one.

[21]  D. Scott Perceptual learning. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[22]  W F Rosenberger,et al.  A sequential design for psychophysical experiments: an application to estimating timing of sensory events. , 1997, Statistics in medicine.

[23]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection theory: A user's guide, 2nd ed. , 2005 .

[24]  Welber Marinovic,et al.  Separable temporal metrics for time perception and anticipatory actions , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[25]  Tomohiro Amemiya,et al.  Tactile duration compression by vibrotactile adaptation , 2010, Neuroreport.

[26]  Derek H. Arnold,et al.  Spatially Localized Distortions of Event Time , 2006, Current Biology.

[27]  A B Kristofferson,et al.  A quantal step function in duration discrimination , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  Thomas H Rammsayer,et al.  Differences in duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals as a function of base duration , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Learning and generalization of time production in humans: rules of transfer across modalities and interval durations , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  D. Picard,et al.  Short-term memory for auditory and visual durations: evidence for selective interference effects , 2011, Psychological Research.

[31]  Massimiliano Di Luca,et al.  Filling the blanks in temporal intervals: the type of filling influences perceived duration and discrimination performance , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[32]  T. Rammsayer,et al.  Neuropharmacological Evidence for Different Timing Mechanisms in Humans , 1999, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[33]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  R. Ivry,et al.  The neural representation of time , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  John Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: the role of cognition, attention, and learning. , 2007, Psychological review.

[36]  David J. Getty,et al.  Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models , 1975 .

[37]  Hugo Merchant,et al.  Do we have a common mechanism for measuring time in the hundreds of millisecond range? Evidence from multiple-interval timing tasks. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Perceptual learning in temporal discrimination: asymmetric cross-modal transfer from audition to vision , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  S. Keele,et al.  Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: a correctional analysis. , 1985, Acta psychologica.

[40]  Monica Gori,et al.  Perceived duration of Visual and Tactile Stimuli Depends on Perceived Speed , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[41]  Michel Treisman,et al.  Combining Information: Probability Summation and Probability Averaging in Detection and Discrimination , 1998 .

[42]  Marc O Ernst,et al.  The Duration of Uncertain Times: Audiovisual Information about Intervals Is Integrated in a Statistically Optimal Fashion , 2014, PloS one.

[43]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Distortions of Subjective Time Perception Within and Across Senses , 2008, PloS one.

[44]  Simon Grondin,et al.  Sensory‐specific clock components and memory mechanisms: investigation with parallel timing , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  F A Wichmann,et al.  Ning for Helpful Comments and Suggestions. This Paper Benefited Con- Siderably from Conscientious Peer Review, and We Thank Our Reviewers the Psychometric Function: I. Fitting, Sampling, and Goodness of Fit , 2001 .

[46]  Nancy Byl,et al.  Practice-Related Improvements in Somatosensory Interval Discrimination Are Temporally Specific But Generalize across Skin Location, Hemisphere, and Modality , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Perceptual learning in auditory temporal discrimination: No evidence for a cross-modal transfer to the visual modality , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[48]  J. T. Walker,et al.  Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones and gaps. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  K. Yarrow,et al.  Humans don't time subsecond intervals like a stopwatch. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  D. Burr,et al.  Auditory dominance over vision in the perception of interval duration , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[52]  Eckart Altenmüller,et al.  Musicians Do Better than Nonmusicians in Both Auditory and Visual Timing Tasks , 2012 .