Electroencephalographic and skin temperature indices of vigilance and inhibitory control

Abstract Neurophysiological markers of the ability to sustain attention and exert inhibitory control of inappropriate responses have usually relied on neuroimaging methods, which are not easily applicable to real-world settings. The current research tested the ability of electroencephalographic and skin temperature markers to predict performance during the Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART), which demands vigilance and inhibitory control. In Experiment 1, we recorded the electroencephalogram (EEG) during the performance of SART and found that event-related potentials underlying inhibitory control (N1 and N2/P3) were influenced by a time on task effect, suggesting a decrement in attentional resources necessary for optimal inhibitory control. In Experiments 2 and 3, we recorded skin temperatures (distal, proximal and the distal-proximal temperature gradient –DPG) and found that they were sensitive to differential demands of mental workload, and that they were related to behavioural performance in the SART. This study suggests that the recording of EEG and skin temperature may be used to monitor fluctuations of attention in natural settings, although further research should clarify the exact psychological interpretation of these physiological indices.

[1]  U. Lorenzo-Seva,et al.  Psychometric Properties of the Spanish Adaptation of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-A for Children , 2008, Psychological reports.

[2]  Joel S. Warm,et al.  Vigilance Requires Hard Mental Work and Is Stressful , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[3]  N. Mackworth The Breakdown of Vigilance during Prolonged Visual Search 1 , 1948 .

[4]  Enric Alvarez,et al.  Psychometric proprieties of Spanish version of Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). , 2012, Actas espanolas de psiquiatria.

[5]  D. Sanabria,et al.  Effects of chronotype and time of day on the vigilance decrement during simulated driving. , 2014, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[6]  D. Dinges,et al.  Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. , 2005, Seminars in neurology.

[7]  Marinus N. Verbaten,et al.  Time effects on event-related brain potentials and vigilance performance , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[8]  Tyler H. Shaw,et al.  Effects of sensory modality on cerebral blood flow velocity during vigilance , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[9]  R. Parasuraman,et al.  A Taxonomic Analysis of Vigilance Performance , 1977 .

[10]  J. Díaz-Morales,et al.  Sleep Habits And Chronotype Effects On Academic And Cognitive Performance İn Spanish Adolescents: A Review , 2016 .

[11]  E. V. van Someren,et al.  Correlated Fluctuations of Daytime Skin Temperature and Vigilance , 2011, Journal of biological rhythms.

[12]  K. Jankowski,et al.  Chronotype and time-of-day effects on mood during school day , 2015, Chronobiology international.

[13]  J. Polich Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[14]  Timothy H. Monk,et al.  A visual analogue scale technique to measure global vigor and affect , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[15]  S. Lockley,et al.  Is 8:30 a.m. Still Too Early to Start School? A 10:00 a.m. School Start Time Improves Health and Performance of Students Aged 13–16 , 2017, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[16]  H. Almirall,et al.  Horne & stberg morningness-eveningness questionnaire: A reduced scale , 1991 .

[17]  M. D. Ernst Permutation Methods: A Basis for Exact Inference , 2004 .

[18]  F. Perrin,et al.  Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  S. Johnstone,et al.  Varying task difficulty in the Go/Nogo task: the effects of inhibitory control, arousal, and perceived effort on ERP components. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[20]  C. Vetter,et al.  Aligning Work and Circadian Time in Shift Workers Improves Sleep and Reduces Circadian Disruption , 2015, Current Biology.

[21]  Pérez,et al.  Propiedades psicométricas de la versión española de la escala Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) , 2012 .

[22]  H. Bokura,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  J. Díaz-Morales,et al.  Daily fluctuations in attention at school considering starting time and chronotype: an exploratory study , 2014, Chronobiology international.

[24]  To stop or not to stop: A high spatio-temporal resolution study of response inhibition using MEG , 2007 .

[25]  Kurt Kräuchi,et al.  The human sleep–wake cycle reconsidered from a thermoregulatory point of view , 2007, Physiology & Behavior.

[26]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Time-Related Changes in Neural Systems Underlying Attention and Arousal During the Performance of an Auditory Vigilance Task , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  L. Lack,et al.  Relationships between the Circadian Rhythms of Finger Temperature, Core Temperature, Sleep Latency, and Subjective Sleepiness , 2004, Journal of biological rhythms.

[28]  E. Wascher,et al.  The Effects of Time on Task in Response Selection - An ERP Study of Mental Fatigue , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[29]  K. Kravchenko,et al.  Application of an Amyloid Beta Oligomer Standard in the sFIDA Assay , 2016, Front. Neurosci..

[30]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[31]  Mirjam Münch,et al.  Gender and age differences in psychomotor vigilance performance under differential sleep pressure conditions , 2006, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  M. Kiefer,et al.  Impulsiveness and ERP components in a Go/Nogo task , 2008, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[33]  Laura Busse,et al.  Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  W. Krijnen,et al.  Lower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[35]  M. Carrillo-de-la-Peña,et al.  The effect of motivational instructions on P300 amplitude , 2000, Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology.

[36]  C. Schmidt,et al.  Time-on-task decrement in vigilance is modulated by inter-individual vulnerability to homeostatic sleep pressure manipulation , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[37]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[38]  C. Czeisler,et al.  Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. , 2002, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[39]  H. Endo,et al.  Mental fatigue and impaired response processes: event-related brain potentials in a Go/NoGo task. , 2009, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[40]  J. A. Madrid,et al.  The Vigilance Decrement in Executive Function Is Attenuated When Individual Chronotypes Perform at Their Optimal Time of Day , 2014, PloS one.

[41]  Benjamin F. Rodriguez,et al.  Psychometric Properties , 2009 .

[42]  E. Vogel,et al.  The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Glenn Gunzelmann,et al.  Relationship of Event-Related Potentials to the Vigilance Decrement , 2018, Front. Psychol..

[44]  Shane M. O’Mara,et al.  Individual differences discriminate event-related potentials but not performance during response inhibition , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[45]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks. , 2013, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[46]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[47]  J. A. Madrid,et al.  Circadian rhythm of wrist temperature in normal-living subjects A candidate of new index of the circadian system , 2008, Physiology & Behavior.

[48]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Time of day, Intellectual Performance, and Behavioral Problems in Morning Versus Evening type Adolescents: Is there a Synchrony Effect? , 2007, Personality and individual differences.

[49]  Hein A.M. Daanen,et al.  Evaluation of wireless determination of skin temperature using iButtons , 2006, Physiology & Behavior.

[50]  James L. Szalma,et al.  The Vigilance Decrement Reflects Limitations in Effortful Attention, Not Mindlessness , 2003, Hum. Factors.

[51]  Daniel J Buysse,et al.  Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: A Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. , 2015, Sleep.

[52]  J. Polich,et al.  Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[53]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  Enrique Molina,et al.  Blue-Enriched White Light Enhances Physiological Arousal But Not Behavioral Performance during Simulated Driving at Early Night , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[55]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[56]  Sally Andrews,et al.  To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[57]  M. Stolarski,et al.  Effects of chronotype and time of day on mood responses to CrossFit training , 2018, Chronobiology international.

[58]  M. Posner,et al.  Spatiotemporal analysis of brain electrical fields , 1994 .

[59]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Two Types of Action Error: Electrophysiological Evidence for Separable Inhibitory and Sustained Attention Neural Mechanisms Producing Error on Go/No-go Tasks , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  S. Folkard Black times: temporal determinants of transport safety. , 1997, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[61]  Judi E. See,et al.  Brain systems of vigilance. , 1998 .

[62]  B. Oken,et al.  Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: physiological basis and measurement , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[63]  ERP components activated by the “GO!” and “WITHHOLD!” conflict in the random Sustained Attention to Response Task , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[64]  Fiona C Baker,et al.  Circadian rhythms, sleep, and the menstrual cycle. , 2007, Sleep medicine.

[65]  M. Wittmann,et al.  Social Jetlag: Misalignment of Biological and Social Time , 2006, Chronobiology international.

[66]  Kurt Kräuchi,et al.  The thermophysiological cascade leading to sleep initiation in relation to phase of entrainment. , 2007, Sleep medicine reviews.

[67]  M. Loeb,et al.  The Psychology of Vigilance , 1982 .

[68]  Anna Wirz-Justice,et al.  Physiology: Warm feet promote the rapid onset of sleep , 1999, Nature.

[69]  W. Helton,et al.  Working memory load and the vigilance decrement , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  J. C. Ballard Computerized assessment of sustained attention: a review of factors affecting vigilance performance. , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.