Blind haste: As light decreases, speeding increases

Worldwide, more than one million people die on the roads each year. A third of these fatal accidents are attributed to speeding, with properties of the individual driver and the environment regarded as key contributing factors. We examine real-world speeding behavior and its interaction with illuminance, an environmental property defined as the luminous flux incident on a surface. Drawing on an analysis of 1.2 million vehicle movements, we show that reduced illuminance levels are associated with increased speeding. This relationship persists when we control for factors known to influence speeding (e.g., fluctuations in traffic volume) and consider proxies of illuminance (e.g., sight distance). Our findings add to a long-standing debate about how the quality of visual conditions affects drivers’ speed perception and driving speed. Policy makers can intervene by educating drivers about the inverse illuminance‒speeding relationship and by testing how improved vehicle headlights and smart road lighting can attenuate speeding.

[1]  C. Cirillo,et al.  Night-time and daytime operating speed distribution in urban arterials , 2016 .

[2]  Guglielmina Mutani,et al.  Effects of Environmental Lighting Conditions on Operating Speeds on Urban Arterials , 2012 .

[3]  Heinrich H Bülthoff,et al.  Foggy perception slows us down , 2012, eLife.

[4]  P. Thompson Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.

[5]  Stuart Anstis,et al.  Moving objects appear to slow down at low contrasts , 2003, Neural Networks.

[6]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Velocity Perception in 3-D Environments , 1996 .

[7]  Heinrich H Bülthoff,et al.  Bayesian motion estimation accounts for a surprising bias in 3D vision , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  Ali S Al-Ghamdi,et al.  Experimental evaluation of fog warning system. , 2007, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[9]  D. A. Owens,et al.  Effects of luminance, blur, and age on nighttime visual guidance: A test of the selective degradation hypothesis , 1999 .

[10]  Thomas A. Dingus,et al.  Peer influence predicts speeding prevalence among teenage drivers. , 2012, Journal of safety research.

[11]  Arthur Huang,et al.  The Effects of Daylight Saving Time on Vehicle Crashes in Minnesota , 2010, Journal of safety research.

[12]  Ingrid van Schagen,et al.  Driving speed and the risk of road crashes: a review. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[13]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[14]  Douglas Coate,et al.  The effects of daylight and daylight saving time on US pedestrian fatalities and motor vehicle occupant fatalities. , 2004, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  Anna Maria Giannini,et al.  Young novice driver subtypes: relationship to driving violations, errors and lapses. , 2010, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[16]  R. Snowden,et al.  Speed perception fogs up as visibility drops , 1998, Nature.

[17]  Justin L. Gardner,et al.  Cortical Correlates of Human Motion Perception Biases , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Marcus R. Munafò,et al.  Dynamic Dazzle Distorts Speed Perception , 2016, PloS one.

[19]  A. Kemeny,et al.  Evaluating perception in driving simulation experiments , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  Ashley Martin,et al.  Speed choice and driving performance in simulated foggy conditions. , 2011, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[21]  Mark S Horswill,et al.  Reducing Contrast Makes Speeds in a Video-Based Driving Simulator Harder to Discriminate as Well as Making Them Appear Slower , 2008, Perception.

[22]  Joanne Wood,et al.  Effects of Reduced Contrast on the Perception and Control of Speed When Driving , 2010, Perception.

[23]  K R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Seeing movement in the dark , 1999, Nature.

[24]  R. Snowden,et al.  The Effect of Contrast upon Perceived Speed: A General Phenomenon? , 1999, Perception.

[25]  W. Frith,et al.  The contribution of alcohol to night time crash risk and other risks of night driving. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  D. Proffitt Embodied Perception and the Economy of Action , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[27]  Michael Mitzenmacher,et al.  Detecting Novel Associations in Large Data Sets , 2011, Science.

[28]  Don Scott,et al.  Car following decisions under three visibility conditions and two speeds tested with a driving simulator. , 2007, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[29]  Rune Elvik,et al.  The Handbook of Road Safety Measures , 2009 .

[30]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[31]  Viola Cavallo,et al.  Distance Perception of Vehicle Rear Lights in Fog , 2001, Hum. Factors.

[32]  J. Edwards,et al.  Speed adjustment of motorway commuter traffic to inclement weather , 1999 .

[33]  George J. Andersen,et al.  Effects of Reduced Visibility from Fog on Car-Following Performance , 2008 .

[34]  S. Hammett,et al.  The effect of luminance on simulated driving speed , 2012, Vision Research.

[35]  Axel Buchner,et al.  Car Backlight Position and Fog Density Bias Observer-Car Distance Estimates and Time-to-Collision Judgments , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[36]  S. Blackburn,et al.  Contrast as a depth cue , 1994, Vision Research.

[37]  Kevin R Brooks,et al.  Simulation of Driving in Low-Visibility Conditions: Does Stereopsis Improve Speed Perception? , 2015, Perception.

[38]  T Assum,et al.  Risk compensation--the case of road lighting. , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[39]  Per Ole Wanvik Effects of road lighting: an analysis based on Dutch accident statistics 1987-2006. , 2009, Accident; analysis and prevention.