Effect of target-distractor similarity on FEF visual selection in the absence of the target

We tested the hypothesis that frontal eye field (FEF) visual activity integrates visual information with a template of a target by examining whether a target that is not present in a search display influences the target selection in FEF. Neural activity was recorded in FEF of macaque monkeys performing visual search for a singleton target defined by color or direction of motion. The target remained constant throughout, but not across experimental sessions. Trials with distractors dissimilar to the target were interleaved with trials with distractors similar to the target. The hypothesis was tested by measuring the magnitude of activity in randomly interleaved trials with the target absent and only distractors in the display. We found that the response to the distractors was significantly greater when presented with displays consisting of distractors that resembled the absent target than when presented with displays consisting of distractors most different from the absent target. The influence of target-distractor similarity on FEF activity was also observed when the target was present, as reported previously. These data suggest that a template of the absent target can influence the selection process in FEF. This provides more direct evidence that FEF integrates visual information and knowledge of the target to determine the goal of a saccade.

[1]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search , 1993, Nature.

[2]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. , 1988, Science.

[4]  J Duncan,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  Hidehiko Komatsu,et al.  A grid system and a microsyringe for single cell recording , 1988, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[6]  H. Pashler Target-distractor discriminability in visual search , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement activation , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  A. Nagy,et al.  Critical color differences determined with a visual search task. , 1990, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[9]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons , 2000, Neuron.

[10]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Chun,et al.  Contextual Cueing: Implicit Learning and Memory of Visual Context Guides Spatial Attention , 1998, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual receptive fields of frontal eye field neurons. , 1973, Brain research.

[13]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Reliability of Macaque Frontal Eye Field Neurons Signaling Saccade Targets during Visual Search , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Findlay Saccade Target Selection During Visual Search , 1997, Vision Research.

[15]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[16]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[17]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. II. Modulation in frontal eye fields specifically related to saccades. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[19]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  M. A. Basso,et al.  Modulation of Neuronal Activity in Superior Colliculus by Changes in Target Probability , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  A Treisman,et al.  Feature analysis in early vision: evidence from search asymmetries. , 1988, Psychological review.

[22]  J D Schall,et al.  Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. III. Memory-contingent visual and saccade responses. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Enhancement of visual responses in monkey striate cortex and frontal eye fields. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  G. Woodman,et al.  Visual Search Remains Efficient when Visual Working Memory is Full , 2001, Psychological science.

[28]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Search Efficiency but Not Response Interference Affects Visual Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2001, Neuron.

[29]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Neural correlates of a decision in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  J. Schall Visuomotor Areas of the Frontal Lobe , 1997 .

[35]  Christos Constantinidis,et al.  The sensory nature of mnemonic representation in the primate prefrontal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  J. Movshon,et al.  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[38]  M. J. Friedlander,et al.  The time course and amplitude of EPSPs evoked at synapses between pairs of CA3/CA1 neurons in the hippocampal slice , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  Michael D'Zmura,et al.  Color in visual search , 1991, Vision Research.

[40]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Saccade target selection in macaque during feature and conjunction visual search , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[42]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques , 1996, Nature.

[43]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[44]  W T Newsome,et al.  Separate signals for target selection and movement specification in the superior colliculus. , 1999, Science.

[45]  Todd S. Horowitz,et al.  Visual search has no memory , 1998, Nature.

[46]  A. Mikami,et al.  Neuronal activity in the frontal eye field of the monkey is modulated while attention is focused on to a stimulus in the peripheral visual field, irrespective of eye movement , 1997, Neuroscience Research.