Rejoinder - Temporal Stochastic Inflation in Choice-Based Research

We examine the specification and interpretation of discrete-choice models used in behavioral theory testing, with a focus on separating “coefficient scale” from “error scale,” particularly over time. Numerous issues raised in the thoughtful commentaries of Louviere and Swait [Louviere, J., J. Swait. 2010. Discussion of “Alleviating the constant stochastic variance assumption in decision research: Theory, measurement, and experimental test.” Marketing Sci.29(1) 18--22] and Hutchinson, Zauberman, and Meyer (HZM) [Hutchinson, J. W., G. Zauberman, R. Meyer. 2010. On the interpretation of temporal inflation parameters in stochastic models of judgment and choice. Marketing Sci.29(1) 23--31] are addressed, specifically the roles of response scaling, preference covariates, actual versus hypothetical consumption, “immediacy,” and heterogeneity, as well as key differences between the experimental setup in Salisbury and Feinberg [Salisbury, L. C., F. M. Feinberg. 2010. Alleviating the constant stochastic variance assumption in decision research: Theory, measurement, and experimental test. Marketing Sci.29(1) 1--17] and those typifying intertemporal choice and construal level theory. We strongly concur with most of the general conclusions put forth by the commentary authors, but we also emphasize a central point made in our research that may have been lost: that the temporal inflation effects observed in our empirical analysis could be attributed to stochastic effects, deterministic influences, or an amalgam; appropriate inferences depend on the nature of one's data and stimuli. We also report on further analyses of our data, as well as a meta-analysis of HZM's Table 1 that is consistent with our original findings. Implications for, and dimensions relevant to, future research on temporal stochastic inflation and its role in choice-based research are discussed.

[1]  Robert Meyer,et al.  Commentary - On the Interpretation of Temporal Inflation Parameters in Stochastic Models of Judgment and Choice , 2010, Mark. Sci..

[2]  Rick L. Andrews,et al.  On the Recoverability of Choice Behaviors with Random Coefficients Choice Models in the Context of Limited Data and Unobserved Effects , 2008, Manag. Sci..

[3]  Nigel Harvey,et al.  Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making , 2004 .

[4]  Peter E. Rossi,et al.  Overcoming Scale Usage Heterogeneity , 2001 .

[5]  Shanefrederick,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference : A Critical Review , 2022 .

[6]  D. McFadden Conditional logit analysis of qualitative choice behavior , 1972 .

[7]  Michael A. Becker Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , 1998 .

[8]  J. Louviere,et al.  The Role of the Scale Parameter in the Estimation and Comparison of Multinomial Logit Models , 1993 .

[9]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Temporal construal. , 2003, Psychological review.

[10]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Diversification bias: Explaining the discrepancy in variety seeking between combined and separated choices , 1995 .

[11]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal Levels and Psychological Distance: Effects on Representation, Prediction, Evaluation, and Behavior. , 2007, Journal of consumer psychology : the official journal of the Society for Consumer Psychology.

[12]  Min Zhao,et al.  Mental Simulation and Preference Consistency over Time: The Role of Process- versus Outcome-Focused Thoughts , 2007 .

[13]  Michel Wedel,et al.  Bayesian Estimation of Circumplex Models Subject to Prior Theory Constraints and Scale-Usage Bias , 2006 .

[14]  Daniel Read,et al.  Chapter 21. Intertemporal Choice , 2008 .

[15]  F. Feinberg,et al.  Modeling Parametric Evolution in a Random Utility Framework , 2005 .

[16]  Steven M. Shugan The Cost Of Thinking , 1980 .

[17]  J. Louviere,et al.  Some probabilistic models of best, worst, and best–worst choices , 2005 .

[18]  Trey Hedden,et al.  Category norms as a function of culture and age: comparisons of item responses to 105 categories by american and chinese adults. , 2004, Psychology and aging.

[19]  Y. Trope,et al.  Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  Y. Trope,et al.  15. Psychological distance , 2007 .

[21]  Denzil G. Fiebig,et al.  The Generalized Multinomial Logit Model: Accounting for Scale and Coefficient Heterogeneity , 2010, Mark. Sci..

[22]  Kamel Jedidi,et al.  Modeling preference evolution in discrete choice models: A Bayesian state-space approach , 2006 .

[23]  J. Stevens Intertemporal Choice , 2013 .

[24]  Jordan Louviere,et al.  Commentary - Discussion of "Alleviating the Constant Stochastic Variance Assumption in Decision Research: Theory, Measurement, and Experimental Test" , 2010, Mark. Sci..

[25]  John G. Lynch,et al.  The Psychology of Intertemporal Discounting: Why are Distant Events Valued Differently from Proximal Ones? , 2005 .

[26]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Construal level theory of intertemporal judgment and decision. , 2003 .

[27]  Jordan J. Louviere,et al.  What If Consumer Experiments Impact Variances as Well as Means? Response Variability as a Behavioral Phenomenon , 2001 .

[28]  Linda Court Salisbury,et al.  Alleviating the Constant Stochastic Variance Assumption in Decision Research: Theory, Measurement, and Experimental Test , 2010, Mark. Sci..

[29]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[30]  M. Keane Modeling Heterogeneity and State Dependence in Consumer Choice Behavior , 1997 .

[31]  Thomas Otter,et al.  Heterogeneity distributions of willingness-to-pay in choice models , 2007 .

[32]  Canan Ulu,et al.  Consuming Now or Later? , 2005, Psychological science.

[33]  I. Simonson The Effect of Purchase Quantity and Timing on Variety-Seeking Behavior , 1990 .