Optimal Multimodal Integration in Spatial Localization
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Robert M. McPeek,et al. Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.
[2] James J. Clark,et al. Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems , 1990 .
[3] W. Geisler,et al. Contributions of ideal observer theory to vision research , 2011, Vision Research.
[4] F. Hamker,et al. A Computational Model for the Influence of Corollary Discharge and Proprioception on the Perisaccadic Mislocalization of Briefly Presented Stimuli in Complete Darkness , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] C. Sherrington. OBSERVATIONS ON THE SENSUAL RÔLE OF THE PROPRIOCEPTIVE NERVE-SUPPLY OF THE EXTRINSIC OCULAR MUSCLES , 1918 .
[6] Daniela Balslev,et al. The University of Birmingham (live System) Eye Position Representation in Human Anterior Parietal Cortex Eye Position Representation in Human Anterior Parietal Cortex , 2022 .
[7] Paul A. Warren,et al. A Bayesian Model of Perceived Head-Centered Velocity during Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement , 2010, Current Biology.
[8] A. A. Skavenski,et al. Contr of eye position in the dark. , 1970, Vision research.
[9] J. Douglas Crawford,et al. Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.
[10] I. Donaldson,et al. The functions of the proprioceptors of the eye muscles. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[11] P. E. Hallett,et al. Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.
[12] H. Bülthoff,et al. Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] Hermann von Helmholtz,et al. Treatise on Physiological Optics , 1962 .
[14] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] W Pieter Medendorp,et al. Spatial constancy mechanisms in motor control , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[16] R. Sperry. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[17] Jean-René Duhamel,et al. Optimal Sensorimotor Control in Eye Movement Sequences , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[18] Christopher J. Peck,et al. The time course of the tonic oculomotor proprioceptive signal in area 3a of somatosensory cortex. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[19] D. Zee,et al. Oculomotor function in the rhesus monkey after deafferentation of the extraocular muscles , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] M. Ernst,et al. Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.
[21] A. A. Skavenski. Inflow as a source of extraretinal eye position information. , 1972, Vision research.
[22] W Li,et al. Visual Direction Is Corrected by a Hybrid Extraretinal Eye Position Signal a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[23] Christopher R Fetsch,et al. Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] Michele Rucci,et al. EyeRIS: A general-purpose system for eye-movement-contingent display control , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[25] M. Hayhoe,et al. Reference frames in saccadic targeting , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[26] R. Wurtz. Neuronal mechanisms of visual stability , 2008, Vision Research.
[27] K. F. Austen,et al. Oculoparalytic Illusion : Visual-Field Dependent Spatial Mislocalizations by Humans Partially Paralyzed with Curare , 2006 .
[28] R. Steinman. Effect of Target Size, Luminance, and Color on Monocular Fixation* , 1965 .
[29] H. Akaike. A new look at the statistical model identification , 1974 .
[30] J. Velay,et al. Shift in saccadic direction induced in humans by proprioceptive manipulation: a comparison between memory-guided and visually guided saccades , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Thérèse Collins,et al. Extraretinal signal metrics in multiple-saccade sequences. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[32] Michael F Land,et al. Vision and the representation of the surroundings in spatial memory , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[33] D. Knill,et al. The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[34] J. Vercher,et al. The role of ocular muscle proprioception in visual localization of targets. , 1990, Science.
[35] Clifford M. Hurvich,et al. Regression and time series model selection in small samples , 1989 .
[36] M. Steinbach,et al. On the role of extraretinal signals for saccade generation , 1995, Experimental Brain Research.
[37] D. Sparks,et al. Corollary discharge provides accurate eye position information to the oculomotor system. , 1983, Science.
[38] G. Lennerstrand,et al. Effects of eye muscle proprioceptive activation on eye position in normal and exotropic subjects , 1997, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.
[39] Arnold Ziesche,et al. Computational models of spatial updating in peri-saccadic perception , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[40] K. Ren,et al. Regulation of spinal neuropeptide genes in a rat model of peripheral inflammation and hyperalgesia. , 1995, Progress in brain research.
[41] Martina Poletti,et al. Stability of the Visual World during Eye Drift , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[42] L. Stark,et al. Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.
[43] Wei Ji Ma,et al. Spiking networks for Bayesian inference and choice , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[44] R. Wurtz,et al. A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.