Soldier Cognitive Processes: Supporting Teleoperated Ground Vehicle Operations

Abstract : Although small ground robotic vehicles provide great promise and benefit to units, there are challenges to overcome if the maximum benefit is to be gained from the introduction of these systems. The robots offer a host of capabilities. However, a person must know how to operate the system procedurally (e.g., use the hand controller buttons, know the menu options) and tactically (e.g., understand what the system can and cannot do). Learning how to operate and employ the ground robotic vehicle imposes a cognitive load on top of other military skills, perceptual decision-making skills, and communication skills that need to be utilized while operating the system. Therefore, trainers and leaders must understand what loads are placed on operators. The major issue is to understand how cognitive load impacts operators performance. Finally, training materials and approaches for operators, especially when the operators have diverse backgrounds and skills, must be designed and developed so operators can effectively and efficiently take advantage of the capabilities offered by the small ground robotic vehicles.

[1]  Kaleb McDowell,et al.  Technological Areas to Improve Soldier Decisiveness: Insights From the Soldier-System Design Perspective , 2012 .

[2]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[3]  J. Sweller COGNITIVE LOAD THEORY, LEARNING DIFFICULTY, AND INSTRUCTIONAL DESIGN , 1994 .

[4]  David E. Kieras,et al.  The Role of a Mental Model in Learning to Operate a Device. , 1984 .

[5]  Philip J. Barnard,et al.  Influencing behaviour via device representation; decreasing performance by increasing instruction , 1990, INTERACT.

[6]  Kim Cardosi,et al.  Pilot-Controller Communication Errors: An Analysis of Aviation Safety Reporting System (ASRS) Reports. , 1998 .

[7]  S. Derry,et al.  Learning from Examples: Instructional Principles from the Worked Examples Research , 2000 .

[8]  Robert A. Wilson,et al.  The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition: How to Situate Cognition , 2008 .

[9]  W. C. Howell,et al.  INSTRUCTIONAL SETS AND SUBJECTIVE CRITERION LEVELS IN A COMPLEX INFORMATION-PROCESSING TASK. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Eliciting Self-Explanations Improves Understanding , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  M. Just,et al.  Constructing mental models of machines from text and diagrams. , 1993 .

[12]  L M Reder,et al.  The role of elaborations in learning a skill from an instructional text , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[13]  David Vernon The Space of Cognitive Vision , 2006, Cognitive Vision Systems.

[14]  M. Lesch,et al.  Driving performance during concurrent cell-phone use: are drivers aware of their performance decrements? , 2004, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  P. Kirschner,et al.  Taking the Load Off a Learner's Mind: Instructional Design for Complex Learning , 2003 .

[16]  V J Gawron,et al.  Performance Effects of Noise Intensity, Psychological Set, and Task Type and complexity , 1982, Human factors.

[17]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Cognitive Psychology: Mind and Brain , 2006 .

[18]  A. Baddeley Is working memory still working? , 2001, The American psychologist.

[19]  R. Catrambone The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. , 1998 .

[20]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Two- and Three-Dimensional Displays for Aviation: A Theoretical and Empirical Comparison , 1993 .

[21]  David F. Feldon,et al.  Cognitive task analysis , 2009 .

[22]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Task Analysis by Problem Solving (TAPS): Uncovering Expert Knowledge to Develop High-Quality Instructional Materials and Training , 2012 .

[23]  R. Catrambone Improving examples to improve transfer to novel problems , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[24]  R CONRAD Speed and Load Stress in a Sensori-Motor Skill * , 1951, British journal of industrial medicine.

[25]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[26]  J. D. Johnson,et al.  Attention and Memory , 1998 .

[27]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  Theories of spatial representations and reference frames: What can configuration errors tell us? , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[28]  R. Mayer,et al.  Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[29]  P. Fitts Cognitive aspects of information processing. 3. Set for speed versus accuracy. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[30]  Rossana de Beni,et al.  Visuospatial working memory in learning from multimedia systems , 2000, J. Comput. Assist. Learn..

[31]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Attention and memory. , 1989, Pediatric annals.

[32]  G. Schöner,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition A Neurobehavioral Model of Flexible Spatial Language Behaviors , 2011 .

[33]  Fred Paas,et al.  Making instructional animations more effective: a cognitive load approach , 2007 .

[34]  Kim M Cardosi An Analysis of En Route Controller-Pilot Voice Communications , 1993 .

[35]  R. A. Brooks,et al.  Intelligence without Representation , 1991, Artif. Intell..

[36]  William B. Rouse,et al.  The effects of type of knowledge upon human problem solving in a process control task , 1985, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[37]  T. Wilson Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious , 2002 .

[38]  Margaret M. Recker,et al.  Learning Strategies and Transfer in the Domain of Programming , 1994 .

[39]  Stephen J. Payne,et al.  Internalizing and the Use Specificity of Device Knowledge , 1993, Hum. Comput. Interact..

[40]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[41]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[42]  K. P. Szlichcinski,et al.  Diagrams and illustrations as aids to problem solving , 1979 .

[43]  David Kelly,et al.  REPORTS BY AIRLINE PILOTS ON AIRPORT SURFACE OPERATIONS (II).. , 1997 .

[44]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures , 2004 .

[45]  D. Touretzky,et al.  Cognitive maps beyond the hippocampus , 1997, Hippocampus.

[46]  R. Catrambone Generalizing Solution Procedures Learned From Examples , 1996 .

[47]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Interactive Example-Based Learning Environments: Using Interactive Elements to Encourage Effective Processing of Worked Examples , 2007 .

[48]  Jessi L. Smith Understanding the Process of Stereotype Threat: A Review of Mediational Variables and New Performance Goal Directions , 2004 .

[49]  Scott A. Shappell,et al.  AIR TRAFFIC CONTROL (ATC) RELATED ACCIDENTS AND INCIDENTS: A HUMAN FACTORS ANALYSIS , 2001 .

[50]  Esa M. Rantanen,et al.  Effect of Air Traffic Controller Taskload and Temporal Awareness on Task Prioritization , 2005 .

[51]  Paul Ginns Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects , 2006 .

[52]  W. C. Howell,et al.  Information processing under contradictory instructional sets. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[53]  Carlos Tena-Tamayo Preventing medical errors in pediatric emergency medicine. , 2005, Pediatric emergency care.

[54]  S. Gronlund,et al.  The distribution of attention and effects on memory for scene expanse , 2010 .

[55]  Krista E. DeLeeuw,et al.  A Comparison of Three Measures of Cognitive Load: Evidence for Separable Measures of Intrinsic, Extraneous, and Germane Load , 2008 .